Dynamic fixation stimuli for visual field testing and therapy

Information

  • Patent Grant
  • 7748846
  • Patent Number
    7,748,846
  • Date Filed
    Wednesday, July 25, 2007
    16 years ago
  • Date Issued
    Tuesday, July 6, 2010
    14 years ago
Abstract
Alteration of a fixation or peripheral stimulus displayed on a computer-driven display allows a human subject to maintain extended visual fixation upon the resulting dynamic stimulus. The fixation is presented upon the display and the stimulus is altered to allow resensitization of the subject's retina, thereby allowing prolonged visual fixation upon the resulting dynamic target. A dynamic stimulus may utilize a frequency doubling illusion.
Description
TECHNICAL FIELD

The present invention relates to systems for maintaining the focus of a subject on a specified region of a display for purposes of testing or improving a subject's vision.


BACKGROUND

The macula is the region of the retina which is used for high acuity vision, as is typically required for reading. To diagnose macular damage, a patient may undergo various types of examinations, including automated perimetry or campimetry, in which the patient is positioned in front of a test surface and is asked to maintain focus on a target. A computer then actuates one or more light sources to present visual stimuli at specific points on the test surface. The patient is asked to press a button in response to perceived test stimuli and the examiner or computer records the patient input and associated spatial information. In this way a visual field map is created.


The human retina is unable to fixate continuously upon an unmoving, unchanging stimulus. After less than a second of stimulation, the subject's retinal cells will adapt to the stimulus and no longer relay any information to the brain regarding the stimulus. Such adaptation is problematic for devices requiring constant fixation upon a fixed point since the viewer must change fixation in order to continue seeing the object of regard. This changing fixation may reduce the accuracy, precision, and overall effectiveness of testing, therapeutic, or non-therapeutic visual stimulation programs that require continuous unchanging visual fixation.


The human visual system includes two simultaneously functioning pathways. The more primitive “M” pathway is dedicated largely to the recognition of moving objects or objects that change in luminosity. The “P” pathway is more evolved and more closely coupled to neural structures associated with conscious thought. The “P” pathway provides recognition of fine detail and color to the brain and is much less committed to motion detection.


In persons who have suffered damage to the visual system (which includes the brain itself), these two pathways frequently are affected to different degrees. Moreover, the two systems usually recover function at different rates. This disparity of functional loss and recovery can cause significant disharmony of the innately matched systems and resulting disturbances to the overall sensory function of the subject.


The ability to accurately detect the different types of visual processing disturbance and to properly treat the affected pathways is of vital importance to the treating practitioner and ultimately, the patient. Increased diagnostic testing sensitivity and visual therapy specificity result in better prognosis for recovery of the individual's function. Current diagnostic testing modalities are limited in their ability to detect specific types of visual function damage.


Most field-of-vision testing falls into one of two categories: Static perimetry and kinetic perimetry. Static perimetry is valuable in establishing a depiction of fine light sensitivity or detail within the central 30 to 60 degrees of visual field (“P” cell function), while kinetic perimetry is valuable in identifying the borders of motion vision function (“M” cell function). However, neither technique is able to identify efficiently the amount or level of motion sensitivity at all the points within the borders.


In static perimetry, the patient fixates upon a specific point while light stimuli (spots) are delivered to various points in the peripheral visual field. Static perimetry can be administered using peripheral stimuli of varying brightness to determine the level of luminance sensitivity throughout the field, or with peripheral stimuli of identical brightness, to screen for the presence or absence of vision at various locations.


Kinetic perimetry also requires a patient to fixate a central spot during delivery of peripheral stimuli. In kinetic perimetry, the stimuli are luminous spots of varying size and brightness. The spots are moved from an area where there is known to be no vision (e.g. the far periphery or physiologic blind spot) toward areas where vision may exist. The patient is tasked with responding upon detection of a moving light in the periphery. Test points are delivered along radii of the circle (or “horopter”) of the patient's field of vision. The points of first detection are recorded on a plot and the circumference of the connected points is considered the border of motion and light sensitivity for the brightness and size of the stimulus used.


Frequency doubling technology (FDT) is used to test the spatial resolution of a subject's visual field. In FDT, a subject attempts to observe a peripheral square or circular grating comprised of striations of alternating luminosities. The gratings are modulated in a wave pattern involving temporal changes in luminosity of the striations. The luminosity modulation is performed above the critical flicker frequency (CFF), typically about 25 Hz, so that the modulation is not noticeable to the subject. If the subject's visual field is sufficient, the subject will observe an optical illusion in which the spatial frequency of the striations is doubled, i.e., the space between the striations is halved. The spatial frequency or contrast may be modulated to determine the limits of the subject's spatial resolution. In a patient with visual field damage, e.g., one suffering from glaucoma, an abnormally low spatial frequency may be required for the subject to observe the frequency doubling illusion. In U.S. Pat. No. 6,068,377, issued May 30, 2000 to McKinnon, an alternate version of FDT is employed, in which the grating is isoluminous, but with alternating hue.


Work in the laboratory of Krystel R. Huxlin at the University of Rochester has utilized a series of spot stimuli for visual field therapy. In the poster presentation, “Training-induced perceptual recovery after visual cortical stroke” Eric Kelts, Jennifer M. Williams, Brad Feldman, Mary Hayhoe and Krystel R. Huxlin, 30th Annual NANOS meeting, Orlando, Fla., 2004, stimuli move with respect to the entire visual field. Such an approach is not consistent with visual restoration therapy approaches that individually target small regions of the visual field. Additionally, movement of a stimulus across a large region will tend to cause distraction of a patient, and is thus inconsistent with visual therapy approaches that utilize a fixation stimulus. If patient feedback were collected with such a system, it would be difficult or even impossible to precisely locate the visual field region that caused the patient's perception to be triggered.


SUMMARY OF THE INVENTION

In accordance with an embodiment of the invention, alteration of a fixation stimulus displayed on a computer-driven display allows a human subject to maintain extended visual fixation upon the resulting dynamic stimulus. The fixation is presented upon the display and the stimulus is altered to allow resensitization of the subject's retina, thereby allowing prolonged visual fixation upon the resulting dynamic target.


In accordance with various embodiments, the steps of presenting the stimulus and altering the stimulus may be repeated for a given number of cycles to allow sustained sensitization and prolonged fixation by the subject. The fixation of the subject may be verified by presenting a test cue and recording a subject's input in response to the test cue. Like the dynamic fixation stimulus, the test cue may also be altered to allow resensitization of the subject's retina.


In order to prevent the subject from predicting the time at which a test cue is presented, successive presentation of the test cue may be performed with varying intervening time intervals. One way to vary the time intervals is to vary the number of cycles of presentation of the fixation stimulus and the altered fixation stimulus.


In accordance with particular embodiments, the alteration of the dynamic fixation stimulus is visible only by using a portion of the subject's retina corresponding to about 2 degrees or less of the subject's central visual field.


There are many suitable fixation stimulus geometries and dynamic transformations. For example, the fixation stimulus may have the form of a repetitively translating object, a rotating object, or an intermingled set of cyclically changing objects. The stimulus could include two sets of intermingled striations of opposite and cyclically altering luminosities.


In embodiments of the invention, the dynamic fixation stimulus is used for purposes of vision testing or training.


In related embodiments, a method is provided for stimulating a motion-detecting visual pathway of a subject. A subject is tasked with fixating on a central stimulus displayed on a computer-driven display. While the subject is visually fixated, a peripheral stimulus is presented on the display. At least one characteristic of the peripheral stimulus is altered so as to trigger the motion-sensitive visual pathway of the subject.


In other related embodiments, the altered characteristic may be the spatial locus, size, pattern, luminosity or hue of the stimulus. The altered and base stimulus may be alternately displayed in a cyclical manner to sustain the display of a dynamic peripheral stimulus. While alternately displaying the base and altered stimulus, the subject's response, indicative of their detection of the stimulus, may be monitored and recorded. The dynamic peripheral stimulus may be, for example, a repetitively translating object, or a repetitively blinking object. By first determining the bounds of a subject's visual field, testing or therapy can be concentrated within those bounds.


In further related embodiments, a computer program product for use on a computer system is provided for stimulating motion-sensitive regions of a subject's peripheral vision. The computer program includes a computer usable medium having computer readable program code, which includes program code for providing a dynamic peripheral stimulation displayed upon a computer-driven display and program code for recording the subject's response to the dynamic peripheral stimulation.


In accordance with another embodiment of the invention, a method is provided for maintaining fixation for purposes of testing or treating the visual field of a subject; the method may also be implemented in a computer system or computer program product. The method includes the steps of providing a fixation stimulus for the subject to visually fixate upon, repeatedly altering the stimulus to create an optical illusion that is only detectable by use of the subject's central visual field and presenting a peripheral stimulus to the subject while the subject is fixated on the fixation stimulus. The subject's response to the peripheral stimulus may be recorded.


In related embodiments, the difficulty level of the optical illusion is tuned to match the spatial perception ability of the subject. The optical illusion may be a frequency doubling optical illusion, such as a frequency doubling grating. The difficulty level of the frequency doubling grating may be tuned by altering the spatial frequency of the grating. For example, the tuning process may include increasing the spatial frequency of the grating until the illusion is not properly detected by the subject and then maintaining the spatial frequency at a level that is detectable by the subject, yet near the limit of the spatial resolution of the subject's central visual field. The fixation stimulus may be systematically altered to target different retinal regions, for example, by rotation or translation, thereby preventing desensitization. A fixation test cue may be presented to query the subject's fixation upon the stimulus.





BRIEF DESCRIPTION OF THE DRAWINGS

The foregoing features of the invention will be more readily understood by reference to the following detailed description, taken with reference to the accompanying drawings, in which:



FIG. 1 shows a flow chart of a method for presenting a dynamic fixation stimulus in accordance with an embodiment of the invention;



FIG. 2 shows a flow chart of a method for presenting a base and alternate dynamic fixation stimulus in accordance with an another embodiment of the invention;



FIG. 3 shows a flow chart of a method for presenting a base and alternate dynamic fixation stimulus in accordance with the embodiment of FIG. 2;



FIGS. 4-10 show a repetitively translating fixation stimulus in accordance with an embodiment of the invention;



FIGS. 11-17 show a rotating fixation stimulus in accordance with another embodiment of the invention;



FIG. 18 shows a grating-like dynamic fixation stimulus in accordance with yet another embodiment of the invention, at a first time point;



FIG. 19 shows a grating-like stimulus in accordance with the embodiment of FIG. 18 at a second time point;



FIG. 20 is a flow chart of a method for creating a dynamic peripheral stimulus and recording a subject's response to the stimulus.



FIG. 21 shows a frequency doubling grating fixation stimulus as displayed on a computerized display;



FIG. 22 shows the frequency doubling grating fixation stimulus of FIG. 21, as perceived by a subject;



FIG. 23 is a flow chart for a method of tuning and using the frequency doubling grating fixation stimulus of FIGS. 21-22.





DETAILED DESCRIPTION OF SPECIFIC EMBODIMENTS

Definitions. As used in this description and the accompanying claims, the following terms shall have the meanings indicated, unless the context otherwise requires:


“Visual Restoration Therapy” shall mean a process for allocating and targeting light stimuli to particular regions of a patient's visual field.


A “subject” shall mean a human receiving light stimuli during a visual restoration therapy session.


In illustrative embodiments of the invention, a dynamic stimulus is presented via a computer-driven display for a human subject (e.g., a patient or trainee) to fixate upon. At least a portion of the dynamic fixation stimulus is periodically altered in appearance, to mitigate the stimulation to corresponding retinal regions and facilitating retinal resensitization. Accordingly, the subject should be able to avoid retinal adaptation and sustain fixation upon the stimulus for longer than would typically be achievable or comfortable with a static stimulus. When used as a central target in perimetry, campimetry, or vision enhancing training (e.g. vision restoration therapy targeting peripheral regions), the dynamic fixation stimulus may help to increase the accuracy, precision, reproducibility, and effectiveness of the procedure. In some embodiments, the dynamic changes in the stimulus may only be observed using the central portion of a subject's visual field. Embodiments may be implemented using a computerized display with appropriate software routines according to methods disclosed herein. Computer based systems for implementing visual restoration therapy are commercialized by the assignee, NovaVision Inc. of Boca Raton, Fla. U.S. Pat. No. 6,464,356 and U.S. Patent Application publication nos. 2005-0213033, 2006-0092377, 2006-0283466, 2007-0038142 all disclose computer based systems for visual restoration therapy and are hereby incorporated by reference herein.


In related embodiments of the invention, a dynamic stimulus is used as a peripheral spot stimulus for purposes of visual field mapping or therapy. The dynamic peripheral stimulus is presented upon a region of a display to target a corresponding peripheral region of the visual field and preferentially activates the motion-detecting cells and/or structures (e.g., the M pathway) in that region of the visual field. When used with visual restoration therapy, perimetry, or campimetry, the dynamic peripheral stimulus allows measurement and/or therapeutic stimulation of motion-detection pathway function. Unlike stimuli of the prior art that move in a pattern across all or large portions of the visual field, dynamic peripheral stimuli of the present invention are confined to subregions of the visual field in a manner that makes them compatible with targeted stimulation of the M pathway in small regions that may be defined or located with respect to a fixation stimulus. Embodiments include using dynamic peripheral stimuli that are “animations” within narrowly bounded zones that are of a size comparable to traditional spot stimuli and moving the bounds of the animation to a second location. Patient responses to the animated stimuli may be recorded upon presentation of each dynamic peripheral stimulus.



FIG. 1 shows a flow chart, which summarizes a method in accordance with an embodiment of the invention. A base fixation stimulus is presented on a computer display (step 100). Although usually presented in the center of the display, the stimulus could also be presented off-center to better target a particular region of the visual field for testing or training. After presenting the base stimulus for a period of time, all or part of the stimulus is altered (step 110) in a manner that should allow resensitization of the subject's retina. The altered stimulus may be presented on a location of the display that is superimposed upon the base stimulus, or slightly offset, so that fixation may be readily maintained on the same area of the display. The area of fixation is typically small, relative to the size of the display (e.g, less than 5% or 1% of the display area). Resensitization may be accomplished by reducing the overall luminosity (brightness), or spectral distribution of luminosity (i.e., color) of all or part of the stimuli, which may advantageously lower stimulation of corresponding retinal regions. After presenting the altered stimulus for a time, one or more additional altered stimuli may be presented, or the base stimulus may presented again by repeating step 100. The resulting loop may be repeated for a number of iterations, until a condition is fulfilled (e.g., completion of testing, or adequate subject performance) or an interrupt is encountered. The timing of the presentation of the base and altered stimuli may be adjusted to minimize retinal desensitization; for example, stimuli may be switched on a sub-second basis. As a result of the repeated resensitizations, that part of the subject's visual field that detects the fixation stimulus should be in a condition of sustained sensitization, thereby allowing for prolonged fixation upon the stimulus.


The repetition between the base and alternate stimuli may cycle, for example, at a frequency between 0.5 and 100 Hz, and more particularly may be between 1 Hz and 10 Hz. For example, the cycle may have a frequency of about 3 Hz, with stimuli presented for period of about 150 ms. However, the cycles need not be regular; the delay between changes in the stimulus may vary from cycle to cycle, but the majority of such delays are typically consistent with the above ranges to allow for sustained sensitization. The delays could increase with time, decrease with time, follow a pseudo-random sequence, or other pattern. For example, the sequence of delays could be 210 ms, 240 ms, 275 ms, 255 ms, etc.



FIGS. 2-3 show flow charts corresponding to embodiments for presenting a dynamic fixation stimulus to a subject and testing the subject for fixation. As in the embodiment of FIG. 1, a loop is employed; a base stimulus is displayed for a time period (step 100), an altered stimulus (step 110) is displayed for a time period, and the process is repeated. After a given number (designated by the variable n) of loop cycles, a fixation test cue is presented (step 200). The test cue may consist of changing the shape, color, pattern or other visually detectable feature of the dynamic fixation stimulus. The subject may be instructed to respond to the test cue through a computer-input device, such as a keyboard, mouse, touchscreen, joystick, microphone, etc. Varying n between cycles of alternately presenting the fixation stimulus and test cue may advantageously cause the test cue to be presented at times that are not readily predictable by the subject, further ensuring subject compliance with regard to fixation. However, the timing of test cue presentation may be varied in other ways, including by altering the duration of base or alternate stimulus presentation. As shown in the flow chart of FIG. 3, the test cue may be altered (step 210) as well, to facilitate continued fixation upon the test cue. Responses may be received from the subject during periods in which either the test cue or altered test cue are displayed. The test cue and altered test cue may be alternately displayed in a loop (steps 200-210) which may repeat a number of times, designated by the variable q. As is true for determining n, q may be determined in a number of ways and may be either fixed or varied. For example, the test cue and alternate test cue may be iteratively displayed (steps 200-210) until a response is received from the subject or may simply be displayed for a given number of cycles, or for a given elapsed time. In any case, after the test cue presentation phase is complete, the base and altered fixation stimulus may again be displayed (steps 100-110).



FIGS. 4-10 schematically show, in time-sequence, an embodiment of a dynamic fixation stimulus and fixation test cue that utilizes a repetitively translating fixation stimulus presented on a computerized display. As shown, the translating stimulus 300 is a green oval which horizontally translates between a first position (position A, shown in FIGS. 4, 6, 7, and 10), and a second position (position B, shown in FIGS. 5, 8 and 9). Although shown as having a higher luminosity than its background 310, the translating stimulus may also be darker than the background. Generally, the translating stimulus should contrast sufficiently with the background so that it is detectable by the subject (though the level of contrast may also be varied during a procedure or course of therapy). Of course, the translating stimulus 300 need not be an oval, but could be a any of a variety of shapes and may translate horizontally, diagonally, or alternately in various directions or patterns. The stimulus 300 may have any of a variety of colors and patterns.


As shown in FIGS. 7 and 8, a fixation test cue 400 may be presented at various times. The subject may be instructed to respond to the appearance of the fixation test cue 400 via a computer input device. In this way, maintenance of visual fixation upon the stimulus 300 may be tracked and/or measured. As shown by way of example, the fixation test cue 400 is a change in color of the translating oval from green to yellow, but could be any change in visually detectable characteristics, including a change in shape or movement-pattern of the stimulus. Optionally, the fixation test cue may also be translated; FIG. 7 shows the fixation test cue in a left (base) position and FIG. 8 shows the fixation test cue in a right (alternate) position. In this way, the fixation test cue may remain visible for a longer period of time.


In accordance with an embodiment of the invention, the timing of the translational movement may be performed at a variety of regular, or irregular timings; by way of example, the timing is based on a regular cycle of 2 Hz. In other words, the stimulus 300 is translated about every 250 ms. The following table lists the position, time and color of the stimulus, and corresponding figures:
















Time
Translational

Corresponding



(ms)
Position
Color
Figure
Comment



















0
Left
Green
4
Base stimulus


250
Right
Green
5
Altered stimulus


500
Left
Green
6


750
Right
Green
Not shown


1000
Left
Green
Not shown


1250
Right
Green
Not shown


1500
Left
Green
Not shown


1750
Right
Green
Not shown


2000
Left
Green
Not shown


2250
Right
Green
Not shown


2500
Left
Green
Not shown


2750
Right
Green
Not shown


3000
Left
Green
Not shown


3250
Right
Green
Not shown


3500
Left
Green
Not shown


3750
Right
Green
Not shown


4000
Left
Yellow
7
Fixation Test Cue


4250
Right
Yellow
8
Fixation Test Cue


4500
Left
Green
9


4750
Right
Green
10 










FIGS. 11-17 show, in time sequence, a rotating fixation stimulus 500, in accordance with another embodiment of the invention. A subject may visually fixate upon a spiked corona 510, which rotates around a central core 520. Although shown with eight regularly arranged spikes 530, the corona 510, may have a greater or fewer number of spikes 530. The spikes may be arranged in either a regular or irregular manner around the core 520. While a subject's vision may saturate with regard to the unchanging parts of the rotating stimulus 500, the alternating positions of the rotating spikes 530 will render at least the spikes 530 continuously visible. A rotating fixation stimulus 500 of altered color (orange corona 210 with a green core 220), may be used as a test cue 600, and is shown in a base configuration in FIG. 15 and in a rotated, altered, configuration in FIG. 16. The corona may rotate in regular or irregular increments; in FIGS. 11-17, each increment of rotation is about 28°. The following table summarizes the illustrative embodiment shown in FIGS. 11-17.


In a specific embodiment, the test cue stimulus 600 is closely matched to the fixation stimulus in terms of luminosity. By closely matching the luminance of the test cue 600 and a fixation stimulus 500, the patient's fixation may be more precisely ascertained, and consequently, the patient may be forced to fixate more precisely upon the stimulus 500. Experimental data show that isoluminous test cues are visible only within about 2 degrees of visual angle. Conversely, if the test cue 600 varied greatly in luminance from the fixation stimulus 500, then a patient could detect the fixation change at greater angles, e.g., 6°, 10°, or even 15° off the line of proper fixation. The luminance of the test cue 600 may vary from the luminance of the base stimulus 500 by a value that is 10%, 5% or less of the base pattern luminance. For example, a test cue stimulus 600 may cycle between colors that are distinguishable when substantially isoluminous; e.g., a yellow test cue stimulus 600 having a luminance of 200 millicandela per square meter and a green fixation stimulus 500 pattern of 190 millicandela per square meter.



















Core





Time
Rotation
520
Corona 510
Corresponding



(ms)
Position
Color
Color
Figure
Comment




















0
Base
Green
Yellow
11



250
Altered
Green
Yellow
12


500
Base
Green
Yellow
13


760
Altered
Green
Yellow
14


1000
Base
Green
Yellow
Not shown


1250
Altered
Green
Yellow
Not shown


1510
Base
Green
Yellow
Not shown


1750
Altered
Green
Yellow
Not shown


2000
Base
Orange
Green
15
Fixation Cue


2250
Altered
Orange
Green
16
Fixation Cue


2500
Base
Green
Yellow
17
Return to







Standard







Fixation







Stimulus










FIGS. 18 and 19 show yet another embodiment in which a striated dynamic fixation stimulus 700 alternates with time between a positive (FIG. 18) and negative (FIG. 19) form. Low-luminosity areas 710 of the stimulus 700 in FIG. 18 have a high luminosity in FIG. 19. Conversely, high-luminosity areas 720 of the stimulus 700 in FIG. 18 have a low luminosity in FIG. 19. The stimulus 700 may repeatedly alternate between these positive and negative forms. If plotted versus time, the displayed luminosity of a given point or area within the stimulus 700 may vary in a square wave, sine wave (which would necessitate the use of intermediate shades of gray), or other repetitive pattern to allow alternate resensitization of saturated retinal regions. As in the preceding examples of FIGS. 4-17, the cycle frequency can vary, but may be optimal when above 0.5 Hz since retinal desensitization may occur after less than one second of stimulation. For example, the striations may alternate between bright and dark every 250 ms (2 Hz). The striations may also continuously or discontinuously rotate around a given point.


If the striations are sufficiently narrow in width, a subject should only be able to resolve the striations by using the central 1-2° of their visual field. As a result, loss of fixation will alter the striated appearance of the stimulus 700. Thus, the subject will be alerted very quickly if his or her fixation has wandered off target. Additionally, such high resolution features may be used with various embodiments of the invention as highly accurate test cues since such test cues should not be detectable using peripheral vision. Use of these test cues may result in increased accuracy in testing subject fixation, and may yield corresponding improvements in testing and training results.


Many variations of the stimulus 700 may be employed. For example, varying colors, patterns or other visually detectable properties may be used, including alternating between more than two such properties (e.g., changing points or regions from red to yellow, or green). While the use of sine waves and square waves described above implies alteration between two fixed levels, the levels may change with time in a pre-defined or semi-random manner.


In related embodiments of the present invention, dynamic stimuli like the dynamic fixation stimuli of FIGS. 1-18 may be used as peripheral spot stimuli for static or kinetic campimetry or perimetry, or as a component of visual restoration therapy (e.g., NovaVision VRT™; NovaVision, Inc, Boca Raton, Fla.).


In embodiments, a subject is tasked with fixating on a central fixation stimulus (static or dynamic) and responding to the appearance of a peripheral spot stimulus. The peripheral spot stimulus has a movement element to the presentation to cause preferential detection by the motion sensitive portion of the visual pathway (a “dynamic peripheral stimulus”). However, the peripheral spot stimulus is still a “spot” in the sense that is presented within bounds that define a fraction of the visual field comparable to the fraction of a visual field that might be stimulated by a static spot stimulus. Thus, the stimulus may be an animation within narrowly defined bounds, i.e., bounds that are substantially less than the visual field of the subject. For example, the bounds may correspond to 5%, 3%, or 1% or the patient's visual filed. The dynamic peripheral stimulus may be a single spot or coherent group of spots or objects. The stimulus may be adjustable in size, brightness, hue, frequency or other parameter. The dynamic peripheral stimulus may be used to map the visual field through perimetry or campimetry, to stimulate visual field region in a visual restoration therapy session, or both. Accordingly, user input may be collected to indicate perception of the dynamic peripheral spot stimuli.



FIG. 20 is a flow diagram for a presentation of the dynamic peripheral spot stimulus. A peripheral location in which the dynamic peripheral spot stimulus is to be applied is selected (step 800). The location may be based on a previous perimetry or campimetry. For example, the border of the motion-sensitive visual field may be determined using kinetic perimetry. Motion sensitive visual-field regions on, near or within this border may then be tested or stimulated for therapeutic purposes using the dynamic peripheral spot stimulus. The overall size of the stimulus may occupy a relatively small fraction of the visual field to increase campimetry resolution or therapeutic specificity. For example, the boundary of the stimulus may consist of 10-20 pixels on a computerized display.


A base peripheral stimulus is presented at the selected location (step 810). Like the above-described fixation stimuli, the stimulus is then altered to target a different retinal region (step 820). The subject's motion-sensitive visual pathway may detect this alteration. Steps 810 and 820 may be repeated a given number of times while awaiting a response from the subject (step 830). Optionally, if there is no response from the subject an additional characteristic (e.g., luminosity, contrast, motion frequency, or hue) of the dynamic peripheral stimulus may be altered; when used for campimetry, a multidimensional motion-sensitive visual-field map may be thereby created. Upon receiving a response from the subject or reaching a time-out or other termination condition, a new location is chosen (step 800). This process (steps 800-830) is repeated until a visual-field map of sufficient detail is created, or until therapy is complete.


By stimulating visual-field regions that do not respond to static stimuli, dynamic peripheral stimuli may be useful for testing and treating subjects that have not optimally responded to other stimuli, and for use with patients having extremely poor vision (e.g., end stage glaucoma, optic atrophy, or retinitis pigmentosa). Dynamic peripheral stimulus may be incorporated as a feature into existing perimeters.


Example One

An isoluminous circular spot of small angular subtense (e.g. 11 pixels on an LCD monitor) is programmed to appear at pre-determined locations within the testing or therapy area of the applicable device. The circular spot of light “blinks” on and off at a pre-determined frequency but does not deviate from its original location.


Example Two

Two small (e.g. 5 pixels on an LCD screen), vertically elongated ellipses of light (paired side-by side) appear at a pre-determined location within the testing or therapy area and alternate illumination at a pre-determined frequency (e.g., right-left-right-left . . . ). The stimulus is modulated in appearance to trigger the percept of motion but the stimulus remains fixed in its location until detection or a pre-determined length of time.


In another embodiment of the invention, a frequency doubling grating may be used as a fixation stimulus in a way that creates an optical illusion. FIG. 21 shows a frequency doubling grating 850 as displayed upon a computer display in a brief moment of time. Like the frequency doubling gratings used for FDT, the spatial luminosity phase of the frequency doubling grating 850 is cycled above the critical flicker frequency, e.g., 25 Hz, to create a frequency doubling optical illusion. FIG. 22 shows how such an optical illusion might appear to a healthy subject. Unlike prior art gratings, however, the grating 850 of FIGS. 21-22 is used as a fixation stimulus, rather than a peripheral test stimulus. Peripheral stimuli used in connection with the grating 850 may be any of those described herein, among others. By appropriately tuning the frequency doubling grating 850, the frequency doubling optical illusion will only be visible to a subject if it illuminates the very central portion of the subject's retina. As a result, very small deviations in fixation will be noticeable to the subject and testable via an appropriate test cue (e.g., a change in the orientation, or other detectable change in the grating 850). For example, due to the structure of the human visual system, movement of the patient's fixation be as little as 2° will result in about a 50% decrease in spatial resolution. With an appropriately configured frequency doubling grating 850, the standard deviation in eye position over the course of therapy may be improved by a factor of about 1-2°. By increasing the mean accuracy of fixation in this manner, the precision and effectiveness of peripheral testing or stimulatory treatment may be improved. When such a dynamic stimulus is used in visual restoration therapy, a greater fraction of the peripheral stimuli may thereby be correctly allocated to appropriate visual field regions.


Due to disease, injury, genetic or other source of variation, different subjects vary in terms of their spatial resolution ability. Therefore, an individualized spatial frequency tuning may be beneficial before beginning testing or training. By tuning the spatial frequency of the grating 850, the subject will be forced to maintain a tighter fixation. FIG. 23 shows a flow chart for a tuning process in accordance with an embodiment of the invention. A spatial frequency parameter controls the spatial frequency of the grating 850 presented on a computerized display. This parameter is initialized to a low frequency starting point, e.g., one that can be detected as frequency doubling illusion by a majority, e.g. 90%, of patients or greater (step 910). The grating 850 is then presented as a frequency doubling fixation stimulus (step 920). If the subject detects the illusion (decision step 930), the spatial frequency parameter is increased and the higher frequency grating 850 is presented again at step 920. This process is repeated until a threshold frequency is reached at which the individual striations of the grating 850 are no longer detectable (decision step 930). The spatial frequency parameter is then decreased to one that is just below the threshold frequency. Accordingly, the spatial frequency adjusted stimulus is then detectable by the subject only with use of the most spatially sensitive region of the subject's visual field. For example, the frequency parameter may be set to be between 2% and 15% lower than the threshold frequency.


Since the grating 850 is presented at a high flicker rate, the subject's retina may be desensitized to the striations. To avoid desensitization, the grating 850 may be systematically perturbed to alternately stimulate different retinal regions; e.g., rotated or translated, as in the embodiments described with reference to FIGS. 1-19. The grating 850 may also be alternated with a fixation test cue, also described above.


In alternative embodiments, the disclosed methods for vision fixation, testing and training may be implemented as a computer program product for use with a computer system. Such implementations may include a series of computer instructions fixed either on a tangible medium, such as a computer readable medium (e.g., a diskette, CD-ROM, ROM, or fixed disk) or transmittable to a computer system, via a modem or other interface device, such as a communications adapter connected to a network over a medium. The medium may be either a tangible medium (e.g., optical or analog communications lines) or a medium implemented with wireless techniques (e.g., microwave, infrared, laser or other transmission techniques). The series of computer instructions embodies all or part of the functionality previously described herein with respect to the system. Those skilled in the art should appreciate that such computer instructions can be written in a number of programming languages for use with many computer architectures or operating systems.


Furthermore, such instructions may be stored in any memory device, such as semiconductor, magnetic, optical or other memory devices, and may be transmitted using any communications technology, such as optical, infrared, microwave, laser or other transmission technologies. It is expected that such a computer program product may be distributed as a removable medium with accompanying printed or electronic documentation (e.g., shrink wrapped software), preloaded with a computer system (e.g., on system ROM or fixed disk), or distributed from a server or electronic bulletin board over the network (e.g., the Internet or World Wide Web). Of course, some embodiments of the invention may be implemented as a combination of both software (e.g., a computer program product) and hardware. Still other embodiments of the invention are implemented as entirely hardware, or entirely software (e.g., a computer program product).


The described embodiments of the invention are intended to be merely exemplary and numerous variations and modifications will be apparent to those skilled in the art. All such variations and modifications are intended to be within the scope of the present invention as defined in the appended claims.

Claims
  • 1. A method for creating a dynamic fixation stimulus for maintaining visual fixation of a human subject, the method comprising: (a) providing, for the subject to visually fixate upon, a fixation stimulus displayed upon a computer-driven display; and(b) altering at least one characteristic of the fixation stimulus to allow resensitization of a retina of the subject; so as to allow prolonged visual fixation,wherein the area occupied by the fixation stimulus and superimposed altered fixation stimulus, is less than 5% of the area of the display.
  • 2. A method in accordance with claim 1, wherein the at least one characteristic is selected from the group consisting of spatial locus, size, luminosity, and color.
  • 3. A method in accordance with claim 1, further comprising: (c) repeating steps (a) and (b) for a given number of cycles.
  • 4. A method in accordance with claim 3, further comprising: (d) presenting a fixation test cue; and(e) recording the subject's input in response to the test cue.
  • 5. A method in accordance with claim 4 comprising performing steps (a)-(e) and repeating step (c) with irregular timing, thereby preventing the subject from reliably predicting the time at which the fixation test cue is presented.
  • 6. A method in accordance with claim 4 further comprising altering the fixation test cue to allow resensitization of the patient's retina.
  • 7. A method in accordance with claim 1 wherein the alteration of the fixation stimulus is detectable only by an area of the subject's retina corresponding to less than 2 degrees of the subject's central visual field.
  • 8. A method in accordance with claim 1, wherein the dynamic fixation stimulus comprises a repetitively translating object.
  • 9. A method in accordance with claim 1, wherein the fixation stimulus comprises a rotating object.
  • 10. A method in accordance with claim 1, wherein the fixation stimulus comprises at least first set and a second set of objects, the objects of the first and second sets being intermingled and contrasting, wherein at least a subset of the objects change appearance over time.
  • 11. A method in accordance with claim 10, wherein the subset of objects change appearance over time in a cyclical manner.
  • 12. A method in accordance with claim 10, wherein the change in appearance is a change in luminosity.
  • 13. A method in accordance with claim 10, wherein the first and second set of objects include sets of striations.
  • 14. A method in accordance with claim 1, further comprising presenting peripheral stimuli for purposes of testing or treatment.
CROSS REFERENCE TO RELATED APPLICATIONS

This patent application claims priority from U.S. Provisional Application Ser. No. 60/833,199 filed on Jul. 25, 2006 and U.S. Provisional Application Ser. No. 60/867,499 filed on Nov. 28, 2006, which are hereby incorporated by reference herein in their entirety.

US Referenced Citations (98)
Number Name Date Kind
1463847 Shilling Aug 1923 A
2213484 Briggs Sep 1940 A
3883234 Lynn et al. May 1975 A
4260227 Munnerlyn et al. Apr 1981 A
4408846 Balliet Oct 1983 A
4421392 Pitts Crick et al. Dec 1983 A
4429961 Sheingorn Feb 1984 A
4533221 Trachtman Aug 1985 A
4660945 Trachtman Apr 1987 A
4679920 Takashi et al. Jul 1987 A
4971434 Ball Nov 1990 A
4995717 Damato Feb 1991 A
5035500 Rorabaugh et al. Jul 1991 A
5050982 Meissner Sep 1991 A
5088810 Galanter et al. Feb 1992 A
5139323 Schillo Aug 1992 A
5147284 Fedorov et al. Sep 1992 A
5191367 Salibello et al. Mar 1993 A
5206671 Eydelman et al. Apr 1993 A
5241332 Farrell Aug 1993 A
5305027 Patterson Apr 1994 A
5321445 Fossetti Jun 1994 A
5325136 Salibello et al. Jun 1994 A
5363154 Galanter et al. Nov 1994 A
5455643 Ki-Ho Oct 1995 A
5534953 Schmielau Jul 1996 A
5539481 Vax Jul 1996 A
5539482 James et al. Jul 1996 A
5550602 Braeuning Aug 1996 A
5565949 Kasha, Jr. Oct 1996 A
5883692 Agonis et al. Mar 1999 A
5886770 Damato Mar 1999 A
5912723 Maddess Jun 1999 A
5946075 Horn Aug 1999 A
5991085 Rallison et al. Nov 1999 A
6061593 Fischell et al. May 2000 A
6062687 Lofgren-Nisser May 2000 A
6286960 Tomita Sep 2001 B1
6290357 Massengill et al. Sep 2001 B1
6321338 Porras et al. Nov 2001 B1
6359601 Maguire, Jr. Mar 2002 B1
6364486 Ball et al. Apr 2002 B1
6386706 McClure et al. May 2002 B1
6406437 Zur et al. Jun 2002 B1
6431708 Krebs Aug 2002 B2
6443977 Jaillet Sep 2002 B1
6464356 Sabel et al. Oct 2002 B1
6519703 Joyce Feb 2003 B1
6540355 Couture Apr 2003 B1
6578966 Fink et al. Jun 2003 B2
6592221 Stregova Jul 2003 B1
6656131 Alster et al. Dec 2003 B2
6688746 Malov Feb 2004 B2
6742892 Liberman Jun 2004 B2
6769770 Fink et al. Aug 2004 B2
6926660 Miller Aug 2005 B2
6990377 Gliner Jan 2006 B2
7004912 Polat Feb 2006 B2
7104659 Grier et al. Sep 2006 B2
7220000 Alster et al. May 2007 B2
7275830 Alster et al. Oct 2007 B2
7309128 Cappo et al. Dec 2007 B2
7367671 Sabel May 2008 B2
20020024634 Fink et al. Feb 2002 A1
20020047987 Massengill et al. Apr 2002 A1
20020087201 Firlik et al. Jul 2002 A1
20020107960 Wetherall et al. Aug 2002 A1
20020186179 Knowles Dec 2002 A1
20030020873 Fink et al. Jan 2003 A1
20030090439 Spitzer et al. May 2003 A1
20030156254 Turovetsky Aug 2003 A1
20030214630 Winterbotham Nov 2003 A1
20040012758 Lin Jan 2004 A1
20040046934 Sponsel et al. Mar 2004 A1
20040051848 Gotze et al. Mar 2004 A1
20040075811 Liberman Apr 2004 A1
20040100616 Eremeev May 2004 A1
20040257528 Miyake et al. Dec 2004 A1
20050001980 Spector Jan 2005 A1
20050004624 Gliner et al. Jan 2005 A1
20050041208 Winterbotham Feb 2005 A1
20050122477 Alster et al. Jun 2005 A1
20050213033 Sabel Sep 2005 A1
20050213034 Nagayoshi Sep 2005 A1
20050213035 Yoshimeki et al. Sep 2005 A1
20060092377 Sabel et al. May 2006 A1
20060283466 Sabel Dec 2006 A1
20060288258 Lo et al. Dec 2006 A1
20060290885 Covannon et al. Dec 2006 A1
20070038142 Todd et al. Feb 2007 A1
20070121070 Alster et al. May 2007 A1
20070171372 Seal et al. Jul 2007 A1
20070182928 Sabel Aug 2007 A1
20070216865 Sabel et al. Sep 2007 A1
20080013047 Todd et al. Jan 2008 A1
20080024724 Todd Jan 2008 A1
20080043201 Todd Feb 2008 A1
20080077437 Mehta et al. Mar 2008 A1
Foreign Referenced Citations (62)
Number Date Country
9305147 Apr 1994 DE
10207839 Sep 2002 DE
115263 Aug 1984 EP
128783 Dec 1984 EP
135736 Aug 1985 EP
0242723 Oct 1987 EP
0537945 Apr 1993 EP
544631 Jun 1993 EP
689822 Jan 1996 EP
775464 May 1997 EP
830839 Mar 1998 EP
1186271 Mar 2002 EP
1236432 Sep 2002 EP
1236433 Sep 2002 EP
1384462 Jan 2004 EP
1402869 Mar 2004 EP
1403680 Mar 2004 EP
1465561 Feb 1977 GB
WO 8000405 Mar 1980 WO
WO 8810088 Dec 1988 WO
WO 9100553 Jan 1991 WO
WO 9110393 Jul 1991 WO
WO 9200037 Jan 1992 WO
WO 9517227 Jun 1995 WO
WO 9700653 Jan 1997 WO
WO 9811819 Mar 1998 WO
WO 9849992 Nov 1998 WO
WO 9952419 Oct 1999 WO
WO 9959461 Nov 1999 WO
WO 0012042 Mar 2000 WO
WO 0036971 Jun 2000 WO
WO 0113859 Mar 2001 WO
WO 0145630 Jun 2001 WO
WO 0147463 Jul 2001 WO
WO 0180808 Nov 2001 WO
WO 0209578 Feb 2002 WO
WO 2002039754 May 2002 WO
WO 2006002070 May 2002 WO
WO 02053072 Jul 2002 WO
WO 03002070 Jan 2003 WO
WO 03002190 Jan 2003 WO
WO 03007944 Jan 2003 WO
WO 03020195 Mar 2003 WO
WO 03041630 May 2003 WO
WO 03065964 Aug 2003 WO
WO 03092482 Nov 2003 WO
WO 03092570 Nov 2003 WO
WO 03098529 Nov 2003 WO
WO 2004066900 Aug 2004 WO
WO 2005004985 Jan 2005 WO
WO 2005037177 Apr 2005 WO
WO 2005044096 May 2005 WO
WO 2005063153 Jul 2005 WO
WO 2005092270 Oct 2005 WO
WO 2005110326 Nov 2005 WO
WO 2005122872 Dec 2005 WO
WO 2006002070 Jan 2006 WO
WO 0239754 Jan 2006 WO
WO 2006006563 Jan 2006 WO
WO 2007003902 Jan 2007 WO
WO 2007109724 Sep 2007 WO
WO 2008077440 Jul 2008 WO
Related Publications (1)
Number Date Country
20080043201 A1 Feb 2008 US
Provisional Applications (2)
Number Date Country
60833199 Jul 2006 US
60867499 Nov 2006 US