Grammar organizer

Information

  • Patent Grant
  • 10902740
  • Patent Number
    10,902,740
  • Date Filed
    Wednesday, July 5, 2017
    7 years ago
  • Date Issued
    Tuesday, January 26, 2021
    4 years ago
Abstract
A teaching and learning aid for articulation of a verb-related language grammar. A number of grammar tables are gathered in structured assemblies. Each assembly describes different verb-related grammar category. A solid body unifies all assemblies into one system by providing one face to each assembly. Each face of a solid body has its assigned color to code its verb-related grammar category. Combination of sides of a solid body visualizes grammar of related colored parts in a sentence. A result is an intuitive and easy-to-use system for teaching, learning, presentation, navigation and analysis of a verb-related grammar.
Description
BACKGROUND—PRIOR ART

The following is prior art that presently appears relevant:














US patents










Patent Number
Code
Issue Date
Patentee





4,478,582
G09B1/34
Oct. 23, 1984
Tucker


4,613,309
G09B19/06
Sep. 23, 1986
McCloskey


5,535,121
G06F17/274
Jul. 9, 1996
Roche et al.


6,022,222
G09B5/065
Feb. 8, 2000
Guinan


2003/0031987
G09B19/04
Feb. 13, 2003
Gore et al.










Foreign patent documents











Foreign Doc. Nr
Cntry Code
Code
Pub. Dt
App or Patentee





202258018U
CN
G09B19/06
May 30, 2012
Feng Zhao


140112
RU
G09B19/06
Apr. 27, 2014
Petr A. Stepichev


2908284
EU
G09B5/12
Aug. 19, 2015
Lee Ju Hwan










Non-patent literature documents





Betty S. Azar, “Understanding and Using English Grammar”,


published by Prentice-Hall, Inc., 1981









This application relates to teaching and learning aids. In particular, it relates to aids for articulation of English language verb-related grammar.


English is a well-structured language. Nevertheless, many students find it difficult to learn. There were attempts to make English learning easier. For example, one can consider a U.S. Pat. No. 4,478,582 to Tucker (1984), U.S. Pat. No. 4,613,309 to McCloskey (1986), U.S. Pat. No. 6,022,222 to Guinan (2000) and US patent #2003/0031987 to Gore et al (2003). Authors try to simplify learning through figures, symbols and codes. Result, however, is far from expected. In fact, it becomes even more complicated.


There is another approach. One can take the most complex part of a language and focus on it. The most complex part of any language is a verb. Professionals know various schemes and charts that help with learning English verbs and tenses. Betty S. Azar's tenses chart from a book “Understanding and Using English Grammar”, FIG. 1 is a widely-recognized example. Similar chart is in US patent #2003/0031987 to Gore et al. in FIG. 2, sheet 2. Innovative teachers modify such charts according to their needs. Example modification is a Chinese patent CN 202258018U to Feng Zhao (2012). However, there is a drawback in all those solutions. They don't provide sentence-building rules inside tenses. A Russian patent RU 140112 to Petr A. Stepichev (2014) is an attempt to approach this problem. In that patent, joined cubic blocks rotate on axis and build sentences. This approach explains sentence-building rules for individual tenses. However, it doesn't produce a systematic picture. It doesn't explain how individual tenses relate to the whole system of English verb-related grammar.


Given above highlights an unmet demand. Students and teachers need a unified system that would picture all verb-related grammar in full variety. There were attempts to build such system. One of examples is a U.S. Pat. No. 5,535,121 to Roche (1996), FIG. 4C, sheet 4 and FIG. 4D, sheet 5. However, this example looks complicated and isn't practical. There is also EU patent #2,908,284 to Lee Ju Hwan (2012) with attempt to address the same problem, FIGS. 1A, 1B, 3, 4, 8. Practical value of such approach is also questionable.


In conclusion, insofar as I am aware, there isn't educational tool for efficient articulation of all verb-related grammar of English language. There is also no system for convenient navigation across numerous verb-related grammar rules.


SUMMARY

All verb-related words of English language form several groups—Grammar Units (GU). Each GU is an item of a structured description of a verb-related grammar for its category. Each GU consists of one reference table or a structured multi-level plurality of reference tables. Each GU sits on a devoted face of a solid body. A solid body serves as a basis for all GUs and makes navigation among GUs easy. A solid body with GUs on it is a system. This system articulates verb-related grammar of English language in its full diversity and makes it navigable. One (but not limited to) of possible embodiments of such system is a Grammar Cube.


ADVANTAGES

Spacial distinction between different faces of the Grammar Cube simplifies verb-related grammar presentation and navigation. It naturally articulates differences between verb-related categories. It also allows articulation of complex grammar structures of sentences. Verb-related parts of sentences bear the same colors as their dedicated faces of the cube. It enhances ability of students to recognize, analyze and learn verb-related parts of a sentence. Abstract grammar theory put on Grammar Cube faces becomes simple. With Grammar Cube as a teaching-aid and learning tool, verb-related grammar teaching and learning comes naturally. Further advantages will become apparent from a study of following description and accompanying figures.





DRAWINGS—FIGURES


FIG. 1—active tenses chart by Betty S. Azar.



FIG. 2—active Finitives Grammar Chart with grammar formulas and examples (Azar's chart with example sentence/phrase for each tense) for action verbs (AV).



FIG. 3—passive Finitives Grammar Chart with grammar formulas and examples for action verbs (AV). Dino shapes show obsolete tenses of English language.



FIG. 4—active Infinitives Grammar Chart for action verbs (AV).



FIG. 5—passive Infinitives Grammar Chart for action verbs (AV).



FIG. 6—active Gerund/Participle 1 Grammar Chart for action verbs (AV).



FIG. 7—passive Gerund/Participle 1 Grammar Chart for action verbs (AV).



FIG. 8—Participle 2 reference table for action verbs (AV).



FIG. 9-14—example matrix for action verbs (AV).



FIG. 15—active Finitives Grammar Chart for verb “to be” (2B) with grammar formulas and examples. Dino shapes show obsoleting tenses.



FIG. 16—active Infinitives Grammar Chart for verb “to be” (2B).



FIG. 17—active Gerund/Participle 1 Grammar Chart for verb “to be” (2B).



FIG. 18—Participle 2 reference table for verb “to be” (2B).



FIG. 19-24—matrix for verb “to be” (2B).



FIG. 25-30—“AUX−” example matrix of modal verbs.



FIG. 31-36—“AUX+” example matrix of modal verbs.



FIG. 37—Grammar Chart for modal verbs.



FIG. 38—example reference table, Infinitives (AV/2B).



FIG. 39—example reference table, Participles 1/Gerunds (AV/2B).



FIG. 40—Grammar Cube sides numbered in 2D.



FIG. 41—Grammar Cube sides numbered in 3D.



FIG. 42—top view of Grammar Cube. Full modal verbs (1)—on, semi-modal verbs (½)—off.



FIG. 43—top view of Grammar Cube. Semi-modal verbs (%)—on, full modal verbs (1)—off.



FIG. 44—3D projection view of Grammar Cube (top-front).



FIG. 45—3D projection view of Grammar Cube (bottom-back).



FIG. 46—aspects sign.



FIG. 47—active voice sign.



FIG. 48—passive voice sign.



FIG. 49—front sign.



FIG. 50—Gerund/Participle 1 sign.



FIG. 51—Participle 2 sign.



FIG. 52—tenses sign



FIG. 53—infinitives sign



FIGS. 54-57—indexed positions of service surfaces on Grammar Cube.



FIG. 58-60—using Grammar Cube as a Present Indefinite Active tense reference (sequence).



FIG. 61—finger swipe movement for showing OD (on-demand) layer.



FIG. 62—algorithm of dialogue windows behavior for Present Indefinite Active tense.



FIG. 63-66—using Grammar Cube as a reference for modal verb “must”.



FIG. 67—algorithm of dialogue windows behavior for modal verb “must”.



FIG. 68—time-line of English tenses.



FIG. 69—whole 246, 248 and tiled 250 faces of Grammar Cube.



FIG. 70—transformed Grammar Cube after TimeStairs function activated (AV). Active voice sides of the cube.



FIG. 71—transformed Grammar Cube after TimeStairs function activated (AV). Passive voice sides of the cube.



FIG. 72—transformed Grammar Cube after TimeStairs function activated (2B). Active voice side of the cube.



FIG. 73—Grammar Cube after TimeStairs function activated (2B). Passive voice side of the cube.



FIG. 74—example of ColorDance function.



FIG. 75—example of educational workspace with Grammar Cube in upper-left corner.



FIG. 76—example of word-processor interface with Grammar Cube.



FIG. 77—example of Grammar Cube in virtual/augmented reality.















DRAWINGS - REFERENCE NUMERALS









Ref#
Part Name
FIG. #





200
main surface
41


202
service bar
41


204
service corner
41


206
1T (top) service bar
54


208
2T (top) service bar
54


210
3T (top) service bar
55


212
4T (top) service bar
56


214
1B (bottom) service bar
54


216
2B (bottom) service bar
54


218
3B (bottom) service bar
55


220
4B (bottom) service bar
56


222
1V (vertical) service bar
54


224
2V (vertical) service bar
55


226
3V (vertical) service bar
56


228
4V (vertical) service bar
57


230
A top service corner
54


232
B top service corner
55


234
C top service corner
56


236
D top service corner
57


238
E bottom service corner
54


240
F bottom service corner
55


242
G bottom service corner
56


244
H bottom service corner
57


246
whole face
69


248
whole face
69


250
tiled face
69


252
tense field (example)
2, 58









DESCRIPTION (FIG. 44 AND FIG. 45)

Classification of Verb-Related Grammar of English Language.


Grammar is a set of rules that describe an architecture of language. A proper representation of verb-related grammar requires proper classification of verbs. A table #1 shows such classification. In this table each verb-related grammar category has its column (e.g., Modals). Rows of the table show levels of organizational structure for each verb-related grammar category (e.g., L2). Rows also show component(s) that build this level (e.g., matrix, FIG. 25-30) and how this level relates to Grammar Unit structure (e.g. Assembly of reference tables).


Intersections of verb-related grammar category columns and level rows produce cells. Each cell contains a figure (e.g., FIG. 9) or a number of figures that describe this cell. Assembly of cells (e.g., L1+L2+L3) of each verb-related grammar category (e.g., Modals) builds a Grammar Unit (GU). Each GU is an item of a structured description of its verb-related grammar category. GU describes distinctive features of its verb-related grammar category by reference tables, grammar formulas, sample sentences, phrases etc.












TABLE #1










Verb-related grammer category















Verbs
Verbals (VL)



















(VB)
In-



Modals





Finitives
finitives
Par 1
Ger
Par 2
(M)





(AV/2B)
(AV/2B)
(AV/2B)
(AV/2B)
(AV/2B)
Modals













L4
Solid Body
Root node
FIG. 41

















(for Grammar Units)






















L3
Grammar
Grammar
Assembly of
FIG.
FIG.
FIG.
FIG.

FIG.



chart
Unit
matrices/
2, 3, 15
4, 5, 16
6, 7, 17
6, 7, 17

37




(GU)
reference











tables











(GU highest











level)








L2
Matrix

Assembly of
FIG.




FIG.





reference
9-14,




25-30,





tables
19-24




31-36


L1
Reference

Reference
FIG.
FIG.
39
39
FIG.
FIG.



table

table.
9
38


8, 18
25-30,





(GU lowest





31-36





level)





















Level
Component
Function & Composition
FIGS.
FIGS.
FIGS.
FIGS.
FIGS.
FIGS.





Explanations to the table #1:


“Finitive” (plural form-“Finitives”)—a term introduced as antonym to a term “Infinitive” (plural form-“Infinitives”). See Glossary for detailed explanation.


Grammar Unit (GU)—assembly of levels from L1 to L3 within one verb-related grammar category.


Root node—foundation for GUs, L4.


Par 1—Participles 1.


Ger—Gerunds.


Par 2—Participle 2.


AV—Action Verbs. All non-modal verbs and all verbals of English language except “to be”


2B—Verb of State. The verb “to be” and its verbals.







1. Finitives


Finitives are the most complex part of English language verbal grammar. This invention treats AV Finitives separately from 2B Finitives. Separation of “to be” from other verbs leads to advantages in English teaching and learning. For example, it becomes clear to students that “to be” does not have passive forms. Another advantage of separation is an opportunity to differentiate obsoleting tenses of “to be” from the ones in active use. Obsoleting tenses on FIG. 15 and FIG. 3 have dino shapes.


1.1 AV Finitives


Grammar Charts on FIG. 2 (Active) and FIG. 3 (Passive) show grammar tenses for AV Finitives. Each grammar tense (e.g., Present Indefinite tense on FIG. 2) has inside set of prescriptive rules for building sentences. A set of standard sentences for a given tense (e.g., Present Indefinite tense, Active voice) as on FIGS. 9-14 according to this invention is a “matrix of a given tense”, or a “Matrix”. Each individual table within a Matrix (e.g., FIG. 9) is a Reference Table (RD. It shows specific type of sentences usage. RT is the first and the lowest level (L1) of its Grammar Unit (GU). Matrix is the second level (L2) of the Grammar Unit (GU). Grammar Chart as on FIG. 2 is the third level (L3) of the Grammar Unit (GU).


Matrices of all AV Finitives both in Active and Passive voices follow AUX+model in all tenses.









TABLE #2







AV Matrix/AUX+











Questions (Q)










Statements
QT1













(S)
G?
QW&G?
S&DJ?
QT2
















+

+

+

+

+















FIG. 9
FIG. 10
FIG. 11, 12
FIG. 13
FIG. 14





Explanation:


AV Matrix-a matrix for AV Finitives.


AUX+-a conjugation model with auxiliary verbs.


In this classification any sentence is either a Question (Q) or a Statement (S).


Any Statement or Question can be either positive (+) or negative (−).


Questions have two groups: Questions Type 1 (QT1) and Questions Type 2 (QT2).


Questions Type 1 (QT1) include following types of questions:


a) General question (G?),


b) General question preceded by Question Word (QW&G?),


c) Disjunctive question. A disjunctive question is a Statement(S) with a disjunctive question tag(DJ?). Abbreviation for this type of questions is S&DJ?.


General questions with Question Words (QW&G?) can have a reversed word order in negative sentence (QW&G?−)R, FIG. 12.


A term “Question Words” unifies individual Question Words (e.g., What, Where, When, How, Why) and Question Phrases (e.g., How many, How much, From where etc.).


Questions Type 2 (QT2) are questions to subject (Q2S).







1.2 2B Finitives


Grammar Chart on FIG. 15 (Active) shows grammar tenses for 2B Finitives. As with AV Finitives, each individual grammar tense of the verb “to be” (e.g., Present Indefinite tense) has inside set of prescriptive rules for building sentences, see FIGS. 19-24.


Verb “to be” is intransitive and doesn't have passive voice. Matrix of 2B Finitive follows mixed AUX model. In Future Indefinite, Future-in-the-Past Indefinite and all Perfect tenses it has auxiliary verbs (AUX+, as in table #2). In Past Indefinite, Present Indefinite, Past Continuous and Present Continuous tenses it does not have auxiliary verbs (AUX−, as in table #3).









TABLE #3







2B Matrix/AUX−











Questions (Q)










Statements
QT1













(S)
G?
QW&G?
S&DJ?
QT2
















+

+

+

+

+















FIG. 19
FIG. 20
FIG. 21, 22
FIG. 23
FIG. 24









Verb “to be” has 2 (two) roles: a) an independent verb, b) a service/auxiliary verb within tense form constructions. Being an independent verb, in Past Indefinite Active tense it has regular, as in table #4, and modified, as in table #5, conjugation forms. Modified form serves for subjunctive and conditional constructions (e.g., If I were you I would go there).









TABLE #4







REGULAR












S+

S−


















I
was
in park
I
wasn't
in park



You
were
in park
You
weren't
in park



He
was
in park
He
wasn't
in park



She
was
in park
She
wasn't
in park



It
was
in park
It
wasn't
in park



We
were
in park
We
weren't
in park



You
were
in park
You
weren't
in park



They
were
in park
They
weren't
in park

















TABLE #5







MODIFIED








S+
S−















(If) I
were
in park
(If) I
weren't
in park


(If) You
were
in park
(If) You
weren't
in park


(If) He
were
in park
(If) He
weren't
in park


(If) She
were
in park
(If) She
weren't
in park


(If) It
were
in park
(If) It
weren't
in park


(If) We
were
in park
(If) We
weren't
in park


(If) You
were
in park
(If) You
weren't
in park


(If) They
were
in park
(If) They
weren't
in park










2. Infinitives


Similar to Finitives, this invention treats AV Infinitives separately from 2B Infinitives. This separation leads to advantages in teaching and learning. Infinitives GU has only 2 (two) levels (L3 and L1) according to the table #1.


2.1 AV Infinitives


Grammar Charts (L3) of AV Infinitives are on FIG. 4 (Active) and FIG. 5 (Passive). There are 2 (two) types of infinitives in English language. One of them is zero (bare) infinitive. Zero (bare) infinitive is an infinitive without “to” in its form. The other one is to-infinitive. To-infinitive has “to” in its form. Both variants are present in FIG. 4 and FIG. 5 in negative and positive. Sample RT of AV Infinitives (L1) is on FIG. 38.


2.2 2B Infinitives


Grammar Chart (L3) of 2B Infinitives is on FIG. 16 (Active). There is no sample RT for 2B Infinitives because it is the same as RT of AV Infinitives.


3. Participles 1/Gerunds


In their grammatical sense Participles 1 and Gerunds are different entities. However, their forms are identical. According to classification provided in the table #1, GU of Participles 1 and Gerunds has 2 levels (L3 and L1).


3.1 AV Participles 1/Gerunds


Grammar Charts of AV Participles 1 and AV Gerunds are on FIG. 6 (Active) and FIG. 7 (Passive). Sample RT is on FIG. 39.


3.2 2B Participles 1/Gerunds


Grammar Charts of 2B Participles 1 and 2B Gerunds are on FIG. 17 (Active). There is no sample RT because it is the same as RT of AV Participles 1/Gerunds.


4. Participle 2


Participle 2 GU has 1 (one) level. Its RT represents a whole GU. There is no division into Active and Passive voices.


4.1 AV Participle 2


RT of AV Participle 2 serves at its Grammar Chart. It is on FIG. 8.


4.2 2B Participle 2


RT of 2B Participle 2 serves at its Grammar Chart. It is on FIG. 18.


5. Modals


Modals GU has 3 levels (L1-L3). There is no division into Active and Passive voices. Sample Grammar Chart (L3) of modal verbs is on FIG. 37. There is no need to put here all Grammar Charts for all modal verbs. It is a common knowledge for English language professionals and educated English speakers. Each tense (e.g. Present tense on FIG. 37) has its own Matrix (L2). The RTs (L1) as on FIGS. 25-30 show sample Matrix for Present Tense of the modal verb “can”.


Modal verbs have two sub-categories:

    • Full-modals—modal verbs that cannot function as Finitives (e.g., can, may, must)
    • Semi-modals—modal verbs that can function both as modal verbs and as Finitives. (e.g., dare, need)









TABLE #6





Modals


















Full (1)
Semi (½)










Matrices of modal verbs represent a mixture of matrix models.

    • Modal verbs like “can”, “may”, “must” follow AUX− model (auxiliary verbs are not used).
    • Modal verbs like “to be able to”, “used to” follow AUX+model (auxiliary verbs are used).
    • Modal verbs like “have to”, “had to” follow mixed model (AUX+/−)









TABLE #7







Matrix (Modals)/AUX−











Questions (Q)










Statements
QT1













(S)
G?
QW&G?
S&DJ?
QT2
















+

+

+

+

+















FIG. 25
FIG. 26
FIG. 27, 28
FIG. 29
FIG. 30
















TABLE #8







Matrix (Modals)/AUX+











Questions (Q)










Statements
QT1













(S)
G?
QW&G?
S&DJ?
QT2
















+

+

+

+

+















FIG. 31
FIG. 32
FIG. 33, 34
FIG. 35
FIG. 36









There is no need in detailed description of individual matrices for each modal verb. It is a common knowledge, familiar to language professionals and educated English speakers.


Imperatives


There are 3 (three) moods in English: 1) Indicative mood, 2) Subjunctive/Conditional mood and 3) Imperative mood. Grammar Units (GU) system articulates any standard verb-related grammar situation in Indicative and Subjunctive/Conditional moods. Imperative mood is an independent supplement to GU classification provided in a table #1. There are simple and complex constructions of Imperatives in English language.


Simple forms of imperative mood are “Do!” (+) and “Don't do!” (−)). There is also imperative form involving “let” (e.g., Let's go! (+)), Let's not go!(−)). Complex imperative constructions like “modal+infinitive” (e.g., You should go!) work as combinations of GUs.


Embodiment—Grammar Cube.


Tables and charts in a table #1 articulate standard verb-related grammar of English language. However, they are numerous. Learning them is a challenge for students of English language. There is a need in a teaching and learning aid that would put them in order and help to navigate among them. A concept of Grammar Organizer (GO) serves this purpose. One of possible (but not limited to) embodiments of GO is a cube. The cube is an organizational core and operational hub for tables and charts. It helps to articulate abstract logic of a language grammar by a natural logic of a geometric figure.


Each face of the cube serves to position one item of a structured description of a verb-related grammar (Grammar Unit). This way the cube organizes all verb-related language grammar into a 3D system. It allows easy navigation between different GUs. Inner structure of each Grammar Unit (GU) serves for navigation inside GU. The cube with GUs on its sides is a Grammar Cube (GC).


Grammar Cube has two kinds of surfaces: main surfaces and service surfaces. Main Surfaces (MS) bear GUs and perform educational function. Service Surfaces (SS) perform both educational and technical functions. A reference numeral 200 shows example of a MS on a FIG. 41. On the same FIG. 41, reference numerals 202 and 204 show examples of Service Surfaces. Each MS of the cube has its number (FIG. 40 and FIG. 41). They are #1-front, #2-left, #3-back, #4-right, #5-top, #6-bottom. Upper and lower parts of MSs #1, 2, 3, 4 bear upper and lower signs. A table #9 details which sign belongs to which MS. Abbreviations of FIG. 52 are Pa—past, Pr—present, Fu—future, FuiP—future-in-the-past, Ind—indefinite, Con—continuous, Per—perfect, P.Con—perfect continuous.















TABLE #9







MS#
1
2
3
4









upper sign
FIG. 52
FIG. 53
FIG. 52
FIG. 53



lower sign
FIG. 49
FIG. 50
FIG. 51
FIG. 50










Main Surfaces of the Grammar Cube have different assigned colors to convey grammar information. The colors are:

    • AV Finitives (yellow),
    • 2B Finitives (pink),
    • AV Infinitives and 2B Infinitives (green),
    • AV Participles 1 and 2B Participles 1 (orange),
    • AV Gerunds and 2B Gerunds (brown),
    • AV Participle 2 and 2B Participle 2 (purple),
    • Modals 1 and Modals % (cyan).


Assigned color for Imperatives is red. Service Surfaces don't have assigned colors.


Grammar Charts of AV and 2B represent equally big parts of verb-related grammar. Each part needs a separate cube. However, having two cubes is not a practical solution. It's more convenient to use one cube but to separate AV and 2B into different modes and to switch between them according on end-user's needs.


In either AV or 2B mode, there are more GU tables to show than Main Surfaces on the cube to accommodate them. A solution is to sort them into permanent and on-demand layers for each mode. Following table details permanent and on-demand layers on the cube for AV mode:














TABLE #10







Category
Chart
MS#*
Layer**




















ACT
Finitives
FIG. 2
1
P



Infinitives
FIG. 4
4
P



Gerunds/Participles I
FIG. 6
4
OD


PASS
Finitives
FIG. 3
3
P



Infinitives
FIG. 5
2
P



Gerunds/Participles I
FIG. 7
2
OD



Participles II
FIG. 8
3
OD





*MS#—Main Surface number.


**Layer—some Grammar Charts stay on MS permanently. They are “permanent layers” (P). Some Grammar Charts appear on MS temporary on-demand. They are “on-demand layers” (OD).


ACT—Active voice


PASS—Passive voice






Following table details permanent and on-demand layers on the cube for 2B mode:














TABLE #11







Category
Charts
MS#
Layer






















ACT
Finitives
FIG. 15
1
P




Infinitives
FIG. 16
4
P




Gerunds/Participles I
FIG. 17
4
OD



PASS




Participles II
FIG. 18
3
OD










Modal verbs stay on MS #5. Design of MS #5 makes both groups of Modals visible but impossible to study at the same time (FIG. 42-43). End-user has to turn up either «1 modals» corner, or «½ modals» corner. It helps to memorize them together but as different entities. Imperatives stay on MS #6 (FIG. 45).


There are two kinds of Service Surfaces (SS) of the Grammar Cube: a) Service Bars (SB) 202 and b) Service Corners (SC) 204. Drawings on FIGS. 54, 55, 56, 57 show basic shapes of the Grammar Cube with reference numerals. Reference numerals detailed in Tables #12 and #13 index Service Surfaces and explain their positions and roles on the cube.











TABLE #12









Service Bars (SB)











Top (T)
Bottom (B)
Vertical (V)














1
Active voice,
empty spacer - 214,
empty spacer - 222,



(FIG. 47) -
(FIG. 54)
(FIG. 54)



206, (FIG. 54)


2
Passive voice,
empty spacer - 216,
aspect sign - 224,



(FIG. 48) -
(FIG. 54)
(FIG. 55)



208, (FIG. 54)


3
Passive voice,
empty spacer - 218,
empty spacer - 226,



(FIG. 48) -
(FIG. 55)
(FIG. 56)



210, (FIG. 55)


4
Active voice,
empty spacer - 220,
aspect sign - 228,



(FIG. 47) -
(FIG. 56)
(FIG. 57)



212, (FIG. 56)
















TABLE #13







Service Corners (SC)










top
bottom







(A) empty spacer - 230,
(E) empty spacer - 238,



(FIG. 54)
(FIG. 54)



(B) empty spacer - 232,
(F) empty spacer - 240,



(FIG. 55)
(FIG. 55)



(C) empty spacer - 234,
(G) empty spacer - 242,



(FIG. 56)
(FIG. 56)



(D) empty spacer - 236,
(H) empty spacer - 244,



(FIG. 57)
(FIG. 57)










Service Bars at the bottom and all Service Corners are left empty with a purpose. Leaving them empty gives room for technical/engineering functionality that may appear necessary (e.g. switched, sockets etc.). There is no sense to describe here technical functionality in its full variety. It doesn't relate directly to the Grammar Cube educational functionality and is a common knowledge for industrial engineers and designers skilled in the art.


Each Main Surface (MS) is either a whole piece 246 or assembly of individual tiles 250. Individual tiles stay on movable blocks. Blocks move forward, backward and tilt their face against their surface of the cube (FIGS. 70, 71, 72). Tiled faces convey grammar information by changing their shape. There are various software and hardware solutions exist to achieve this result. There is no need to give detailed technical descriptions for these solutions because it is a common knowledge for professionals skilled in the art.


Default configuration Grammar Cube has a following view:

    • a) Active Finitives, Active Infinitives and Modals—FIG. 44
    • b) Passive Finitives, Passive Infinitives and Imperatives—FIG. 45

      Operation (FIGS. 58-61, 63-66, 70-74)


Grammar Cube operates in 3 (three) types of environments, each one having its advantages:

    • 1. Physical Reality (PhR)


In PhR Grammar Cube works as a hardware system. With hardware Grammar Cube students literally have first-hand/tactile experience of English grammar.

    • 2. Virtual Reality (VR)


In VR Grammar Cube works as a software. Its advantage is an opportunity of quick implementation across various operation systems. It is also an affordable solution with global reach.

    • 3. Augmented Reality (AR) AR allows combination of VR and PhR benefits.


      1. PhR Version.


Operation of a hardware version of Grammar Cube is similar to operation of a smartphone. Each face of the cube works as a smartphone screen. It responds to standard interface gestures (e.g., tap, double-tap, swipe etc.). In its hardware (PhR) version, Grammar Cube works as a standalone product and together with other device(s) (e.g., computer, smartphone, tablet PC, smart whiteboard etc.).


Examples of Hardware Grammar Cube Usage:


a) Educational Tool for Studying Verbal Forms and Tenses.


In its default configuration Grammar Cube shows reference grammar materials on its sides. Teachers and students use them for teaching/learning English grammar. For example, each grammar tense field (e.g., tense field 252 of Active tenses chart FIG. 2, MS #1) shows its formula. Each tense field is also a tap field. One-time tap on the tense field (FIG. 58) expands it into a new dialogue window, providing end-user with next step of choice (FIG. 59) until a Reference Table opens (FIG. 60). Sample algorithm of dialogue windows behavior for AV Present Indefinite Active tense is on FIG. 62. Abbreviations on FIG. 62 are S—sentence, Q—question, Q2O—question to object, Q2S—question to subject, G—general question, QW&G—general question preceded by a question word, DJ—disjunctive question. Other abbreviations in the Reference Tables were explained before. There is no need to give detailed algorithms for each tense. They follow the same logic of a sample algorithm on FIG. 62. Double-tap closes dialogue window at any step.


Tapping on “Modals 1” field (FIG. 63) shows Pa, Pr, Fu and FuiP forms of one full modal verb. Further tapping on “Modals 1” field scrolls related list of modal verbs and their forms. There is no need to give list of modal verbs here. It is a common knowledge for English language professionals and educated speakers. Once modal verb is found, end-user taps tense field of this verb (FIG. 64) to go through dialogue window(s) (FIG. 65) to a target Reference Table (FIG. 66). Double-tap closes dialogue window at any step. To see Reference Tables of semi-modal verbs end-user taps on “Modals ½” field and does the same steps as described for “Modal 1”.


Swiping finger up on MSs #2, 3, 4 (FIG. 61) brings on-demand (OD) layers. Double-tap takes OD layer down. Tables #10 and #11 detail OD layers and their functions.


b) Educational Tool for Positions of Tenses on a Timeline (TimeStairs Function).


Different tenses in English language have different positions on a timeline (FIG. 68). It takes extra effort for a teacher to explain that Past Perfect is “deeper” in time than Present Indefinite. Another difficulty is explanation of Present Perfect tense. It is often mixed with Past Indefinite tense. Students don't understand why Present Perfect is called “present” when it denotes past event(s).


To address this issue, Grammar Cube has a dedicated function. It is a TimeStairs function (TS). When end-user activates this function, faces of the Grammar Cube produce changing of their shape. Changing of shape conveys grammar information about position of different tenses on a timeline. There is a number of ways to activate/deactivate this function and produce mechanical change of a shape. There is no need to give full description of all engineering solutions. It is a common knowledge for skilled in the art mechanical engineers.


The following transformations happen after activation of this function:

    • on a MS #1 (AV, active voice), see FIG. 70:
      • all Future-in-the-Past tenses, Present Indefinite and Present Continuous tenses remain on their places.
      • all Future tenses go forward.
      • Past Indefinite and Past Continuous go back.
      • Past Perfect and Past Perfect Continuous go back deeper than Past Indefinite and Past Continuous. This way they show that they are the deepest tenses on a time-line.
      • Present Perfect and Present Perfect Continuous tilt and form a slope. This way they show that from one side they relate to present but from another side they have connection with past.
    • on MS #3 (AV, passive voice), see FIG. 71:
      • obsolete tenses (all Perfect Continuous tenses, Future Continuous and Future-in-the-Past Continuous) are inactive.
      • Future-in-the-Past Indefinite, Future-in-the-Past Perfect tenses remain on their places.
      • Future Indefinite and Future Perfect move forward.
      • Present Perfect tilts and forms a slope.
      • Past Indefinite and Past Continuous go back.
      • Past Perfect goes back deeper than Past Indefinite and Past Continuous.


In 2B mode:

    • on MS #1 (2B, active voice), see FIG. 72:
      • obsoleting tenses of the verb “to be” (all Perfect Continuous tenses, Future Continuous and Future-in-the-Past Continuous) are inactive.
      • Present Indefinite, Present Continuous, Future-in-the-Past Indefinite and Future-in-the-Past Perfect tenses remain on their places.
      • Future Indefinite and Future Perfect move forward.
      • Present Perfect tilts and forms a slope.
      • Past Indefinite and Past Continuous go back.
      • Past Perfect goes back deeper than Past Indefinite and Past Continuous to show that it is the deepest tense on a time-line.
    • verb “to be” doesn't have passive voice. Because of that, MS #3 in 2B mode remains inactive and empty, see FIG. 73.


      c) A 3D Projector of a Verb-Related Grammar in a Sentence (ColorDance Function).


Recognition of verbal forms and tense structures in sentences is one of the goals of grammar studies. ColorDance function addresses this issue. Grammar Cube works as a colored grammar display in 3D. For example, a sentence “I could have told them but I didn't.” displays on the sides of Grammar Cube as (see FIG. 74): “I could [MS #5, Past field of “can”-CYAN] have told [MS #4, AV mode, Perfect Infinitive field—GREEN] them but I didn't [MS #1, AV mode, Past Indefinite field—YELLOW]”. Verbal parts of a sentence bear colors of related sides of the cube: “I could [CYAN] have told [GREEN] them but I didn't [YELLOW]”. ColorDance function makes grammar visible for students.


2. VR Version.


In VR version Grammar Cube is a software model that performs the same functions as a hardware model. Software model runs on computers, tablet PCs, smartphones, game consoles, smart watches, hologram projectors, interactive boards, VR headgear and other computing devices. End-user interacts with Grammar Cube through keyboard, mouse, joystick, touch-pad, touch-screen, digitizer-pad, motion sensing, wired glove(s), voice commands and any other input interface.


Extra Functions:






    • a) Educational course navigation tool. It helps to see a general level of student's progress. A sample design of such educational course interface is on FIG. 75. The cube is in upper left corner. At the beginning of a course all parts of the cube are inactive and gray. Following student's progress, individual parts of the cube become active and colored. They show how much grammar student already knows. Comparing inactive (gray) and activated (colored) parts of the cube, a student tracks current level of progress.

    • b) A feature of word-processing software. Having Grammar Cube as a part of a word-processing tools allows 3D analysis of verb-related forms and tenses in a text as on FIG. 76.


      3. AR Version





Augmented reality (AR) version combines functionality of PhR and VR. AR version runs on any device that supports AR technology (smartphone, tablet PC, Google Glass, Microsoft Hololens, AR contact lenses, Virtual Retinal Display etc.). Using AR allows building more simple hardware Grammar Cube to offer it at a lower price. It is cheaper to simulate complex transformations feature like TimeStairs in AR, than to build it in hardware. End-user holds hardware cube model in hands and interacts with AR menus and functions (FIG. 77).


CONCLUSIONS, RAMIFICATIONS AND SCOPE

From provided description a reader will see that usage of Grammar Cube makes grammar teaching, learning, presentation, navigation and analysis efficient. Complicated and obscure grammar theory becomes visible, clear and alive. Students naturally engage in educational process. They build better grammar skills with less time and effort. Grammar Cube focuses students' attention to the core of a grammar course. A teacher shows more and talks less. It decreases teacher's burden in class, saves lesson time and makes teaching more productive.


While descriptions given above contain many details, these should not be construed as limitations on the scope, but rather as an exemplification of embodiment(s) thereof. Other ramifications are possible. Languages with grammar structure different from English can employ different figures for organization. Other multi-faceted solids can serve as a basement for grammar arrangement.


Principle of a language grammar organization by usage of a multi-faceted solid has numerous applications in grammar theory and practice. It builds a foundation of a new branch of linguistic science—“Polytopial grammar” or “Polyhedral grammar”. Choice of figures (solids) for each case in each language depends on grammar complexity, end-users convenience, authors choice and other reasons that influence final result. Accordingly, the scope should be determined not by the embodiment(s) illustrated, but by the appended claims and their legal equivalents.


GLOSSARY





    • Finitive (plural form—“Finitives”)—a term introduced in this invention to denote grammatical tense as “time+aspect” (e.g., present continuous, past perfect). Introduction of a special term became necessary to avoid misunderstanding between “tense” as “time” (e.g., present, past, future) and “tense” as “time+aspect” (e.g., present continuous, past perfect). Finitive is an antonym to a term “Infinitive”.

    • reference table (RD—a table showing prescriptive rules for specific type of sentences (e.g., FIG. 9). The lowest level (L1) in a Grammar Unit (see table #1).

    • matrix—a structured assembly of reference tables. It shows a standard set of prescriptive rules for building sentences within a given verb-related grammar category (e.g., FIG. 9-14). Level 2 (L2) in a grammar unit (see table #1).

    • grammar chart—a structured assembly of matrices (e.g., FIG. 2). Level 3 (L3) in grammar unit (see table #1).

    • grammar unit (GU)—an item of a structured description for a given verb-related grammar category. Assembly of levels from L1 to L3 within one verb-related grammar category in the table #1 (e.g., AV Finitives GU, L1+L2+L3). It describes distinctive features of its verb-related grammar category by reference tables, grammar formulas, sample sentences etc.

    • Grammar Cube—a solid body with grammar units on its faces. Top level (L4) in a structural arrangement (see table #1).

    • grammar category/category of words—part of speech (e.g., verb, noun, adjective etc.).

    • category of verb-related grammar/verb-related grammar category—sub-division of verbs and verbals by category (e.g., infinitives, modal verbs, participles, etc.).

    • Action Verbs (AV)—in this invention means all non-modal verbs and all verbals of English language except the verb “to be”. Abbreviation for Action Verbs is AV.

    • Verb of State (2B)—in this invention verb of state means the verb “to be” and its verbals.





Abbreviation for the verb “to be” is 2B.

    • field—any cell of any grammar chart on Grammar Cube.
    • tense field—a cell of Active/Passive Grammar Chart.
    • layer—information shown on Grammar Cube face(s).
    • AUX+—denotes a conjugation model with auxiliary verbs (e.g., They don't read).
    • AUX−—denotes a conjugation model without auxiliary verbs (e.g., She mustn't read).
    • Questions Type 1 (QT1)—questions to object.
    • Questions Type 2 (QT2, Q2S)—questions to subject.
    • G?—a general question.
    • QW&G?—a general question preceded by question word(s)
    • S&DJ?—a disjunctive question.
    • 0+—zero (bare) form of Infinitive/Gerund/Participle 1, positive.
    • 0−—zero (bare) form of Infinitive/Gerund/Participle 1, negative.
    • 1+—“to” form of Infinitive/Gerund/Participle 1, positive.
    • 1−—“to” form of Infinitive/Gerund/Participle 1, negative.
    • AV layers set/AV mode—combination of layers describing Action Verbs (AV).
    • 2B layers set/2B mode—combination of layers describing the verb “to be” (2B).
    • permanent layer—layer that constantly stays on a Grammar Cube.
    • on-demand layer—layer that temporary appears above a permanent layer.
    • cube surface/main surface—face of a side of Grammar Cube.
    • service surface/minor surface—face on edge or vertex of Grammar Cube.
    • service bar—edge surface of Grammar Cube.
    • service corner—vertex surface of Grammar Cube.
    • TimeStairs function (TS)—transformations of Grammar Cube, showing positions of tenses on a time-line.
    • Sequence of Tenses function (SoT)—a visual depiction of grammar rules of sequence of tenses.
    • ColorDance function—function for projection of a verb-related grammar of a sentence on related parts of Grammar Cube and Grammar Navigator.
    • Grammar Player—a grammar playback function.
    • whole face—a Grammar Cube face that acts as one piece.
    • tiled face—a Grammar Cube face that acts as assembly of pieces (tiles).
    • tile—smallest unit of a tiled face.
    • verbal form—non-tense form of a verb (e.g., indefinite, gerund, participle %).
    • tense/tense form—tense related form of a verb.
    • verbal structure/construction—combination of verbal categories/tenses/forms in a sentence.

Claims
  • 1. A system for articulation of a verb-related language grammar, comprising: a. a cubical body which includes a plurality of faces; wherein each face includes a display means configured to display a plurality of structured description of verb-related language grammar;each face includes an area displaying verb-related language grammar configured to response to gestures, wherein said structured description on the area can be manipulated through said gestures;wherein each face is configured to display an assigned color to code its verb-related grammar category;a built-in microphone configured to recognize voice commands;the cubical body further includes speech processing features;a grammar player which includes player controls for manipulating the playback of the audio of the structured descriptions;the grammar player includes multiple operating modes: an assignment mode and a demonstration mode;b. said structured description of said verb-related language grammar includes a plurality of fields which comprises: reference tables, grammar formulas and sample sentences; a student is prompted to select one or more said fields to create a sentence or a phrase;c. a teacher tool which allows teachers to customize said structured description of said verb-related language grammar according to the needs of the student; said teacher tool includes: a word-processing software interface with said cubical body; wherein said reference tables, grammar formulas and sample sentences are customizable utilizing the word-processing software; andan interactive board remotely communicate with the cubical body;d. a feedback module for providing one or more visual feedback based on the sentence or the phrase created by the student with said cubical body; ande. an educational course navigation tool for tracking a student's level of progress by enabling individual parts of the cube become active and colored.
  • 2. The system for articulation of a verb-related language grammar of claim 1, wherein the gestures include tap, double-tap, swipe and click.
  • 3. The system for articulation of a verb-related language grammar of claim 1, wherein the player controls includes play, block-by-block play, stop, pause and playback speed slider.
  • 4. The system for articulation of a verb-related language grammar of claim 1, wherein the speech processing features includes a speech to text feature and a text to speech feature.
  • 5. The system for articulation of a verb-related language grammar of claim 1, wherein said displaying means include interactive boards and displays, Augmented Reality (AR)/Virtual Reality (VR) helmets and goggles, holographic and retinal displays.
  • 6. The system for articulation of a verb-related language grammar of claim 1, wherein cubical body includes two type of surfaces: main surface (MS) and service surface (SS); wherein MS shows Grammar Units (GUs) for performing educational function and SS performs both educational and technical functions.
CROSS-REFERENCE TO RELATED APPLICATION

This application claims the benefit of provisional patent application Ser. No. 62/358,606, filed 2016 Jul. 6 by the present inventor.

US Referenced Citations (22)
Number Name Date Kind
3728800 Magram Apr 1973 A
4044476 Marsh Aug 1977 A
4419080 Erwin Dec 1983 A
4470821 LeCapelain Sep 1984 A
4478582 Tucker Oct 1984 A
4613309 McCloskey Sep 1986 A
5013245 Benedict May 1991 A
5429371 Bledsoe Jul 1995 A
6659774 Ramos Dec 2003 B1
7238107 McVay, III Jul 2007 B2
20010001631 Platts May 2001 A1
20030031987 Gore Feb 2003 A1
20050069845 Spaventa Mar 2005 A1
20070269773 Slade Nov 2007 A1
20080233546 Baker Sep 2008 A1
20100001469 Hubbard Jan 2010 A1
20100230897 Stott Sep 2010 A1
20120015333 Self Jan 2012 A1
20120098199 Scriven Apr 2012 A1
20120115112 Purushotma May 2012 A1
20160129338 Almutawa May 2016 A1
20180008882 Mehta Jan 2018 A1
Related Publications (1)
Number Date Country
20200135040 A1 Apr 2020 US
Provisional Applications (1)
Number Date Country
62358606 Jul 2016 US