The present invention relates to the intranasal administration of oxytocin (OT), especially for the modulation of social cognition and/or behavior, being mental and/or behavioral operations underlying social interactions, and also to the intranasal administration of other peptides, including Orexin-A, especially for the treatment of narcolepsy, and insulin, especially for the treatment of diabetes.
A growing body of evidence demonstrates a role of OT in social cognition and behavior1-3. For instance, a single administration of OT has increased empathy4-5, trust6, group-serving behaviours7-8, sensitivity of eye gaze9, and theory-of-mind performance in healthy individuals10 and in patients with psychiatric disorders11. OT has also been proposed as a novel therapy for disorders characterized by social dysfunction, such as autism and schizophrenia spectrum disorders12-13.
Despite initial promise, however, recent work has either failed to identify changes in social behavior after OT administration14 or has provided results that are only significant for specific subgroups or contexts15. These mixed results have been largely attributed to such contextual and individual differences16, and factors that may influence biological activity of exogenous OT have yet to be thoroughly investigated16-18.
The present inventors postulate that other factors to dose and delivery method may influence biological activity of exogenous OT, and similarly to other peptides, including Orexin-A and insulin.
Olfactory nerve fibres innervate a limited segment of the deep upper narrow nasal passage, while the trigeminal nerve provides sensory and parasympathetic innervation to the deep upper and posterior segments of the nose. Drug transport along these cranial nerve fibres may offer a potential direct route to the central nervous system (CNS)15,23 circumventing the blood-brain barrier (BBB), and this segment is not adequately targeted by conventional nasal spray devices15,26.
The present inventors postulate that, by virtue of nose-to-brain activity, the targeted intranasal administration of OT to this innervated segment of the nasal passage could enable pharmacodynamic effects in the brain disproportionate to what would be achieved by absorption into the blood, and that this method of targeted delivery may improve the reliability, therapeutic index, and effect magnitude of OT treatment effects due to improved drug deposition15,31-32.
An unchallenged assumption in the literature that would benefit from closer experimental scrutiny in humans is that intranasal administration is the best means of delivering OT to modulate social cognition and behaviour15.
Despite early work demonstrating that intravenous (IV) administration can influence social behavior and cognition33-34—presumably via blood absorption and subsequent action across the BBB—subsequent human studies assessing the effect of OT on cognitive functions have used methods that deliver OT via the nasal cavity. Although there is a strong theoretical basis that intranasal delivery is a more appropriate means of administering OT, a controlled comparison of pharmacodynamics (PD) effects after intranasal (i.e., nose-to-brain) and intravenous (i.e., transportation across the BBB) administration has not been done.
Furthermore, in relation to the dosing regimen, the majority of intranasal OT studies have evaluated between 20 and 40 international units (IU)36. There is no comprehensive empirical evidence substantiating this dosage37-38, though successful in other disciplines (e.g. obstetrics)39. This is despite the negative long-term effects of OT treatment observed in non-human adolescent mammals40, and the presence of OT and cross-reactive vasopressin (AVP) receptors throughout the body41 that are involved in a variety of homeostatic functions related to observed side effects42.
It is an aim of the present invention to provide for improved efficacy in the intranasal administration of oxytocin (OT), especially for the modulation of social cognition and/or behavior, and other peptides, including Orexin-A, especially for the treatment of narcolepsy, and insulin, especially for the treatment of diabetes.
In one aspect the present invention provides a method of modulating conditions relating to social cognition and/or behaviour in a human subject using oxytocin, non-peptide agonists thereof and/or antagonists thereof, comprising: providing a nosepiece to a first nasal cavity of the subject; and administering less than 24 IU of oxytocin, non-peptide agonists thereof and/or antagonists thereof to an upper region posterior of the nasal valve which is innervated by the trigeminal nerve.
In another aspect the present invention provides a method of modulating a condition in a human subject using a peptide, non-peptide agonists thereof and/or antagonists thereof, comprising: providing a nosepiece to a first nasal cavity of the subject; and administering less than 24 IU of a peptide, non-peptide agonists thereof and/or antagonists thereof through the nosepiece to an upper region posterior of the nasal valve which is innervated by the trigeminal nerve.
In a further aspect the present invention provides a nosepiece for delivering substance to a nasal cavity of a subject, the nosepiece comprising: a first, inner body part; and a second, outer body part which is disposed about at least a distal portion of the inner body part and defines a tip; wherein the inner body part comprises a base portion which defines a flow passage therethrough, and a projection at the distal end thereof which supports the tip and confers a rigidity in the sagittal direction, which enables the tip to open fleshy tissue at an upper region of the nasal valve and thereby expand an open area of the nasal valve, and a flexibility in a lateral direction, orthogonal to the sagittal plane, which facilitates insertion of the tip into the nasal valve.
In a yet further aspect the present invention provides a nosepiece for delivering substance to a nasal cavity of a subject, the nosepiece comprising a body part which comprises a base portion which defines a flow passage therethrough, and a projection at a distal end of the base portion which at least in part provides a tip of the nosepiece and confers a rigidity in the sagittal direction, which enables the tip to open fleshy tissue at an upper region of the nasal valve and thereby expand an open area of the nasal valve, and a flexibility in a lateral direction, orthogonal to the sagittal plane, which facilitates insertion of the tip into the nasal valve.
Preferred embodiments of the present invention will now be described hereinbelow by way of example only with reference to the accompanying drawings, in which:
Device
The delivery device comprises a housing 115, a nosepiece 117 for fitting in a nasal cavity of a subject, a mouthpiece 119 into which the subject in use exhales, such as to enable delivery of an air flow into and through the nasal airway of the subject on exhalation by the subject through the mouthpiece 119, and a delivery unit 120, which is manually actuatable to deliver substance to the nasal cavity of the subject.
The housing 115 comprises a body member 121, in this embodiment of substantially elongate, tubular section which includes an aperture 123 at one end thereof, through which projects an actuating part of the delivery unit 120, in this embodiment as defined by the base of a substance-containing chamber 173 of a substance-supply unit 169.
The housing 115 further comprises a valve assembly 127 which is fluidly connected to the nosepiece 117 and the mouthpiece 119, and operable between closed and open configurations, as illustrated in
The valve assembly 127 comprises a main, body element 128 which includes a valve seat 129 defining a valve opening 130, and a valve element 131 which is movably disposed to the body element 128 between closed and open positions, as illustrated in
As particularly illustrated in
The valve element 131 comprises an elongate arm 141, in this embodiment a flexible arm, one end 145, in this embodiment the lower end, of which is pivoted to the pivot 135 of the body element 128, and the other, upper end 147 of which slideably engages the sliding surface 137 of the body element 128, and a valve member 149 which is supported by the arm 141.
In this embodiment the arm 141 comprises a first, here lower, arm section 151, which is biased, here inwardly, such that, when the valve element 131 is in the closed, rest position, the lower arm section 151 is inclined inwardly relative to the longitudinal axis of the housing 115 and engageable by the substance-supply unit 169 when manually actuated to move the valve element 131 to the open position, as will be described in more detail hereinbelow.
In this embodiment the arm 141 further comprises a second, here upper, arm section 153, which engages the sliding surface 137 of the body element 128 and acts to bias the valve element 131 to the closed position.
In this embodiment the valve member 149 comprises a seal 161, in this embodiment a flexible or resilient element, which acts to close the valve opening 130 as defined by the valve seat 129 when the valve element 131 is in the closed position, and a support 163 which supports a central region of the seal 161.
With this configuration, where the seal 161 is centrally supported, when the valve element 131 is moved to the open position, the support 163 biases the central region of the seal 161, causing the seal 161 to bulge outwardly in this central region and thus provide that the seal 161 engages the valve seat 129 only at the peripheral edge of the seal 161, until the point is reached when the seal 161 is suddenly and explosively released from the valve seat 129.
This mode of release is believed to be particularly effective in the present application where it is desired to achieve a sudden, initial burst of air flow, in that substantially the entire sealing surface of the seal 161 is released in one instant, which compares to an alternative mode of a peeling-type release, where a smaller section of a sealing surface is released, followed by the remainder of the sealing surface, which tends to provide a smaller initial burst pressure.
In this embodiment the delivery unit 120 comprises an outlet unit 167 for delivering substance into the nasal airway of the subject, and a substance-supply unit 169 for delivering substance to the outlet unit 167.
In this embodiment the outlet unit 167 comprises a nozzle 171 for delivering substance to the nasal airway of the subject. In this embodiment the nozzle 171 is configured to provide an aerosol spray. In an alternative embodiment, for the delivery of a liquid, the nozzle 171 could be configured to deliver a liquid jet as a column of liquid.
In a preferred embodiment the distal end of the outlet unit 167 is configured to extend at least about 2 cm, preferably at least about 3 cm, and more preferably from about 2 cm to about 3 cm, into the nasal cavity of the subject.
In this embodiment the substance supply unit 169 is a pump unit, which comprises a substance-containing chamber 173 which contains substance and extends from the aperture 123 in the housing 115 as the actuating part of the substance-supply unit 169, and a mechanical delivery pump 175 which is actuatable, here by depression of the substance-containing chamber 173, typically by a finger or thumb of the subject, to deliver a metered dose of substance from the substance-containing chamber 173 to the outlet unit 167 and from the nozzle 171 thereof, here as an aerosol spray.
In this embodiment the substance-containing chamber 173, when depressed to actuate the substance supply unit 169, engages the lower arm section 151 of the arm 141 of the valve element 131, such as simultaneously to provide for actuation of the substance-supply unit 169 and opening of the seal 161 of the valve element 131, whereby substance, here in the form of a spray, and an air flow, here as a burst of air, are simultaneously delivered to the nasal cavity of the subject.
In this embodiment the mechanical delivery pump 175 is a liquid delivery pump for delivering a metered dose of substance.
In this embodiment the substance-supply unit 169 is a multi-dose unit for delivering a plurality of metered doses of substance in successive delivery operations.
In this embodiment the housing 115 further comprises a sealing member 181, here an annular seal, in the form of an O-ring, which slideably receives the substance-containing chamber 173 of the substance-supply unit 169, such as to prevent the escape of the delivered air flow from the aperture 123 in the housing 115.
As particularly illustrated in
In this embodiment the inner body part 202 is formed of an acrylonitrile butadiene styrene (ABS) plastic, here Guardian/Lustran® ABS 308 (as supplied by Ineos ABS (USA) Corporation).
In this embodiment the outer body part 204 is formed of a thermoplastic elastomer (TPE), here Versaflex® OM 1040X-1 (as supplied by GLS/PolyOne Corporation), having a Shore A hardness of 42.
As particularly illustrated in
In this embodiment the distal, forwardmost end of the base portion 208 defines a surface 210 which tapers or is inclined in relation to the longitudinal axis of the nosepiece 117, such that the surface 210 of the base portion 208 is inclined in an direction away from the distal end of the projection 212, and the base portion 208 is shorter at that side which is opposite to the projection 212.
The projection 112 is configured to confer a rigidity in the sagittal direction, which enables the tip 206 of the nosepiece 117 to open the fleshy tissue at upper region of the nasal valve and thereby expand the open area of the nasal valve, and a flexibility in the lateral direction, which facilitates insertion of the tip 206 of the nosepiece 117 into the nasal valve. In this embodiment, from measurement by acoustic rhinometry (AR), the nosepiece 117 provides for expansion of the area of the nasal valve to an area which is at least twice the area of the nasal valve when unexpanded and in a rest state.
In this embodiment the projection 212 extends axially in substantially parallel relation to the longitudinal axis of the nosepiece 117.
In this embodiment the projection 212 has the form of a blade, with a length d1 in the sagittal direction being greater than a length d2 in the lateral direction.
In this embodiment the length d1 in the sagittal direction is 1.5 times greater than the mean length d2 in the lateral direction.
In one embodiment the length d1 in the sagittal direction is 1.7 times greater than the mean length d2 in the lateral direction.
In this embodiment the length d1 in the sagittal direction is 1.9 times greater than the mean length d2 in the lateral direction.
In this embodiment the length d1 in the sagittal direction is 2 times greater than the mean length d2 in the lateral direction.
In this embodiment the projection 212 has a length d1 in the sagittal direction of about 2 mm.
In this embodiment the projection 212 has a length d2 in the lateral direction of about 1 mm.
In this embodiment the projection 212 has a main body section 214 and a tip section 216 which has a shorter length d3 in the sagittal direction than the length d1 of the main body section 214, here defining a step at an inner edge thereof.
In this embodiment the projection 212 has a tapering lateral cross-section along its length, with the length d2 in the lateral direction reducing in cross-section along its length towards the distal end.
In this embodiment the length d2 in the lateral direction reduces from about 1.1 mm to about 0.8 mm from the proximal to the distal end of the projection 212.
Study
A randomized, double-blind, double-dummy, crossover study was performed, in which 18 healthy male adults were randomly assigned, and 16 completed four single-dose treatments; these being (1) the intranasal administration of a liquid spray of 8 IU of OT delivered using the device of
This study compared pharmacodynamic (PD) effect of OT on social cognition and behavior, as indexed by the presentation of emotional stimuli and in particular amygdala activity.
In order to examine the neural correlates of OT's behavioral and cognitive effects, researchers have adopted brain-imaging tools such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Converging evidence from this field suggests the amygdala, a key brain region for emotion regulation86, processing87 and detection113, is an important target of OT administration. The modulation of amygdala activity in response to emotional stimuli is arguably the most replicated and well-characterized result within brain imaging and intranasal OT studies88,89,114-117. Irrespective of this prior work, however, it is not clear how OT travels to the brain or which OT dose is more likely to modulate the recruitment of amygdala during the presentation of emotional stimuli. By comparing amygdala activity after both intranasal and intravenous OT administration, when comparable blood levels are achieved, research can determine if neural modulation occurs via direct nose-to-brain transport (as currently assumed) or through systemically circulating OT crossing the BBB. There is both animal70 and human33-34 research to suggest systemic OT can influence social behavior and cognition—however, research has not yet evaluated amygdala activity after intravenous delivery with an intranasal OT comparator.
Recent theories also underscore OT's role in the facilitation of approach-related behaviours118 and the modulation of social stimuli salience16. Given the established relationship between cognitive resource allocation and pupil dilation119-120, pupilometry offers a non-invasive neurobiological measure of engagement towards emotional stimuli. Research indicates that intranasal OT enhances pupil dilation55 and the salience of social cues121. However, the relationship between amygdala activity and pupil-indexed cognitive engagement has yet to be explored and may contribute to a better understanding of the effects of OT.
Primary outcomes were the evaluation of facial emotional expression, in particular in relation to amygdala activity, and secondary outcomes included pharmacokinetic (PK) profiles and ratings of trustworthiness.
This study hypothesized a main effect of the administration of 8 IU-OT and 24 IU-OT on the perceived intensity of anger, and that this effect would be more pronounced with ambiguous emotional stimuli compared to stimuli with less ambiguous emotional expressions.
This study examined dose-dependent effects of 8 IU-OT and 24 IU-OT.
This study also investigated the impact of OT on trust ratings of the same facial stimuli.
In order to characterize PK and evaluate potentially different relationships between PK and PD by method of drug delivery, the time course of blood plasma concentrations of OT and physiologically interacting substances vasopressin (AVP) and cortisol were measured following treatment. Modulation of social cognition after 8 IU-OT and 24 IU-OT administration, but not after IV-OT producing comparable blood exposure, would provide evidence that 8 IU-OT and 24 IU-OT administration is, at least in part, directly acting on the brain rather than across the BBB.
Eligible participants were males between the ages of 18 to 35, in good physical and mental health. Exclusion criteria included use of any medications within the last 14 days, history of alcohol or drug abuse, clinically relevant history of physical (including renal, cardiac, endocrine, pulmonary, hepatic, nervous, gastrointestinal, hematological and metabolic disorders), or psychiatric illness, and IQ<75. Fifty-seven male volunteers were assessed for eligibility, and 18 participants were selected aged 20-30 years (M=23.81, SD=3.33). Two participants withdrew after enrollment [1 withdrew after the first session, and the other withdrew after completing three sessions], and data from these participants is not included in the analyses.
A screening visit occurred between 3-21 days prior to randomization. The Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence52 and the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview53 were used to index IQ and confirm the absence of psychiatric illness, respectively. A physical examination was performed, including ECG and the collection of routine blood samples. In addition, an otolaryngologist confirmed normal nasal anatomy and patency in participants (via physical examination) and acoustic rhinometry (AR) data were collected (SRE 2000; Rhinometrics, Lynge, Denmark). Three measures were calculated from the AR data: Minimum cross-sectional area (MCA; i.e., the narrowest section of the nasal cavity), total volume from nostril to 5 cm deep (TV0-5), and total volume from 2-5 cm deep (TV2-5).
A randomized, placebo-controlled, double-blind, double-dummy, four-period crossover design was used for this study. Participants were randomized to one of four treatment sequences, using a four-period four-treatment Latin square method (ACDB-BDCA-CBAD-DABC in a 4:4:4:4 ratio), with a period of at least six days between treatments to prevent potential carry-over effects. Both the participants and research team were blinded to treatment using visually matching devices and IV apparatus during data collection.
In this study, the delivery device capitalizes on two aspects of nasal anatomy to facilitate delivery to the respiratory and nasal epithelia32. Firstly, as the user is blowing through the mouth against a resistance, the soft palate automatically closes, isolating the nasal cavity from the oral cavity, preventing lung deposition and limiting gastrointestinal deposition23. Secondly, in conjunction with closure of the soft palate, an optimized nosepiece is employed that allows deeper insertion to directs the exhaled breath and OT into the upper-posterior nasal cavity segments23.
The 8 IU-OT, 24 IU-OT and Placebo formulations were supplied by Sigma-Tau Industrie Farmaceutiche Riunite S.p.A. The Placebo formulation was 0.9% sodium chloride.
The IV-OT formulation was supplied by AS Grindeks, Riga, Latvia was supplied as a 10 IU/ml formulation and added to a 0.9% sodium chloride solution for infusion shortly before administration (600 ml/hour over 20 minutes). The intravenous dosage and infusion rate was chosen so as to generate peripheral OT concentrations that are equivalent to intranasal delivery, as confirmed by experiment.
In order to ensure appropriate use and standardization, participants were trained on the use of the intranasal delivery device by watching a demonstration video, following written instructions, and administering practice saline sprays under the supervision of trained research staff during the screening session.
At the beginning of each experimental session, exclusion and inclusion criteria were confirmed and the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory 54 was administered. Blood samples were taken to assess routine measures and acoustic rhinometry (AR) was performed (per procedures during screening) to confirm that the nasal cavity environment did not significantly differ between sessions due to nasal cycles24.
Participants completed the social-cognitive task 40 minutes after treatment in a magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scanner while functional MRI and physiology data was recorded.
Participants were presented with visual stimuli through MRI-compatible goggles (VisualSystem; NordicNeuroLab, Bergen, Norway) using E-Prime 2.0 (Psychology Software Tools, PA, USA), and responded using a grip response collection system (ResponseGrip, NordicNeuroLab, Bergen, Norway).
Participants were presented with 20 male and 20 female faces55 displaying angry, happy, and emotionally ambiguous facial expressions [derived from the Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces database56] and 20 images of geometrical shapes. The social-cognitive task consisted of five blocks of 20 trials, as illustrated in
For the evaluation of the faces, participants were asked a first question (Q1) which was either: How angry is this person? (anchors: not angry-very angry) or, How happy is this person? (anchors: not happy-very happy), and a second question (Q2), which was always the same: How much would you trust this person? (anchors: not at all-very much). For both questions, participants were asked to rank their answer on a visual analogue scale (VAS) from 1 to 5, with location of the cursor on the VAS randomized on the presentation of each question. Mean ratings for each of the questions were averaged per session within each of the emotional categories, yielding seven behavioral variables (Q1: Happy face—happy, Happy face—angry, ambiguous face—happy, ambiguous face—angry, angry face—happy, angry face—angry; Q2; Trust). These stimuli and questions were chosen to assess three levels of emotion perception; ambiguous, non-ambiguous with corresponding cues and ratings (e.g., angry ratings on angry ratings), and non-ambiguous with conflicting cues and ratings (e.g., angry ratings of happy faces).
For the evaluation of the shapes, participants were asked either: (Q1) How yellow is this shape? (anchors: not yellow-very yellow) or How blue is this shape? (anchors: not yellow-very yellow). Q2 was always: How much do you like this color? (anchors: not at all-very much). In the same manner as for ranking the faces, participants were asked to rank their answer on a visual analogue scale (VAS) from 1 to 5, with location of the cursor on the VAS randomized on the presentation of each question.
Brain imaging data was collected on a 3T General Electric Signa HDxt scanner with an 8-channel head coil (GE Healthcare, Milwaukee, Wis., USA).
In the acquisition of MRI data, the protocol included a T2*-weighted gradient echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence acquired in the transverse plane with the following parameters: Repetition time (TR)=2400 ms, echo time (TE)=30 ms, flip angle (FA)=90°, 64×64 matrix. One run of 528 volumes was collected for each individual in each OT condition (48 slices; in-plane resolution 3.75×3.75 mm; slice thickness 3.2 mm, no gap). A T1-weighted volume, used for co-registration purposes, was acquired using a sagittal fast spoiled gradient echo (FSPGR) sequence with the following parameters: TR=7.8 ms, TE=2.9 ms, FA=12°, 166 slices; in-plane resolution: 1×1, slice thickness: 1.2 mm, 256×256 matrix.
Pupilometry data was collected using an MR-compatible coil-mounted infrared EyeTracking system (NNL EyeTracking Camera®, NordicNeuroLab, Bergen, Norway) at a sampling rate of 60 Hz. Data was recorded using the iView X Software (SensoMotoric Instruments, Teltow, Germany), with a trigger from the stimulus computer syncing the onset of the pupilometry recording to stimulus presentations.
During the experimental sessions, blood samples were collected via IV catheter to assess peripheral levels of OT, AVP, and cortisol at baseline and five time points after the completion of the 20-minute IV administration (0 mins, 10 mins, 30 mins, 60 mins, and 120 mins) throughout the session. Blood samples were centrifuged at 4° C. within 20 minutes of blood draw, after which plasma was frozen at −80° C. until enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA) using commercially available kits (Enzo Life Sciences, Farmingdale, N.Y.) was performed using standard techniques (including sample extraction).
Pharmacodynamic Analysis
Analysis was conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics version 22 (IBM Inc.) to determine pharmacokinetics and examine the impact of treatment on outcome measures. A linear mixed-model (LMM) approach was adopted58, congruent with a recent intranasal crossover psychotropic drug trial95, for the analysis of emotional expression evaluation, pharmacokinetics, state anxiety, and trustworthiness. All models were fitted using an unstructured matrix. For any significant main effects (i.e., p<0.05), post-hoc tests were performed with the adjustment of critical p values to correct for multiple comparisons using a 5% false discovery rate (FDR)59.
Experimental treatment was both a fixed and repeated effect in a LMM to assess the impact of treatment on emotion and trustworthiness ratings.
Additionally, in order to investigate the impact of treatment on blood plasma OT, AVP, cortisol concentration and state anxiety a LMM was fitted with 3 fixed factors (treatment, time, treatment×time), 1 repeated factor (treatment). In order to investigate if nasal environments changed between treatment conditions, a repeated measures MANOVA was performed with three dependent variables; MCA, TV0-5, and TV2-5.
Participant responses to the task are presented in Table 1. Due to equipment difficulties, data was not collected during two (out of sixty-four) testing sessions. A LMM revealed a significant main effect of treatment in the ratings of anger when presented ambiguous faces [F(3,14.72)=7.62, p=0.003;
In order to evaluate the specificity of the effect for ambiguous faces (vs. non-ambiguous faces with corresponding cues and non-ambiguous with conflicting cues), a percentage change score was calculated comparing ratings after 8 IU-OT and Placebo treatments, and comparing 8 IU-OT with 24 IU-OT treatments (i.e., the treatment comparisons that demonstrated significant differences in emotional ratings). Ambiguous=anger ratings of ambiguous faces; NA—corresponding=Anger ratings of non-ambiguous faces with corresponding cues; NA—conflicting=Anger ratings of non-ambiguous faces with conflicting cues. Stimuli category was both a fixed and repeated effect in a LMM to assess the impact of stimuli category on the reduction of anger ratings. For the LMM comparing percentage change between the 8 IU-OT and Placebo treatment, there was a main effect for stimuli type [F(2,14.42)=4.79, p=0.025;
Out of 384 possible data points, 19 OT, 26 AVP, and 18 cortisol plasma concentration assessments were excluded due to technical issues relating to blood sample collection or analysis.
Oxytocin Blood Plasma Concentration:
The mean OT plasma concentrations over time after the administration of 8 IU-OT, 24 IU-OT, IV-OT and Placebo (with error bars representing standard error of the mean) are represented in Table 2 and
Vasopressin Blood Plasma Concentration:
The mean AVP plasma concentrations over time after the administration of 8 IU-OT, 24 IU-OT, IV-OT and Placebo (with error bars representing standard error of the mean) are represented in Table 3 and
Cortisol Blood Plasma Concentration:
The mean cortisol plasma concentrations over time after the administration of 8 IU-OT, 24 IU-OT, IV-OT and Placebo (with error bars representing standard error of the mean) are represented in Table 4 and
In this study, it has been demonstrated that 8 IU-OT treatment reduces the perception of anger in emotionally ambiguous facial stimuli with minimal systemic exposure. Importantly, the current findings are the first to suggest that a low dose of OT is more effective than a higher dose in modulating social cognition. Moreover, these results provide behavioral evidence that OT delivered intranasally using the delivery device of this study reaches the brain and influences social cognition, whereas peripherally administered OT, which similarly increased plasma OT concentration, had no such effect.
This data highlights the subtle effect of OT on the processing of emotionally ambiguous facial stimuli in relation to anger perception, as there was no difference in the ratings of angry or happy faces. Whereas the specific effects of OT in the emotionally ambiguous stimuli indicate that OT only influences the emotional assessment of stimuli which are non-abundant with overt cues, the lack of effects in the happy and angry stimuli could also be explained by the relatively low variability in ratings of these stimuli. Notably, there were also no differences in ratings of trust between the placebo condition and any of the OT conditions. While this may have been due to the explicit nature of the “trust” question [most research has used more nuanced economic tasks64], this adds to mounting evidence that OT may not increase the perception of trustworthiness96-97.
The present delivery regime, which provides for efficacy with lower dose concentrations, also has a particular advantage of enabling regulation of the balance of OT and AVP concentrations49 via cross-reactivity with AVP receptors50,98-100. In addition, compared to higher doses, lower doses have been shown to increase peripheral levels of OT in saliva65, attenuate cortisol stress responses66, and increase eye gaze in patients with Fragile X syndrome67. Furthermore, a low dose of OT administered shortly after birth has been shown to increase partner preference later in life68. Similarly, lower doses have been associated with stronger increases in social recognition compared to higher doses69-70.
Much like OT, AVP receptors are located both centrally and peripherally74-75 and play an important role in social behavior and psychopathology49. It is postulated that this “off target” activity may contribute to a non-linear dose-response and further highlights the importance of establishing the dose regimen that optimizes therapeutic effects101.
Importantly, the present dose-response data provides evidence to the optimal dose for social cognition modulation, demonstrating that a lower dose is more likely to modulate social cognition than a higher dose. Furthermore, patients with underlying deficits responsive to OT, may respond more robustly than healthy volunteers.
The present data on the perception of facial stimuli is generally consistent with results from past studies in humans, particularly negatively valenced emotions81, as differences were only discovered on the perception of anger in emotionally ambiguous faces. These results documenting specifically reduced negativity bias for emotionally ambiguous faces have important implications for disorders that are characterized by a negative bias towards social stimuli (e.g., social anxiety disorder). Prior studies suggest that OT reduces bias towards negative information in clinically anxious82 and high trait anxious individuals83; however, this is the first study to the present inventors' knowledge to report data suggesting a reduction of negativity bias in healthy individuals.
Nasal Valve Dimension Analysis
Analysis was conducted using the R statistical package (version 3.1.1; R Development Core Team, 2014) to examine the role of the cross-sectional area of the nasal valve, being the slit-like structure at the junction between the anterior and posterior regions of each nasal cavity, on pharmacodynamics. A repeated-measures ANOVA was first conducted to investigate if the cross-sectional area of the nasal valve significantly fluctuated from session-to-session (screening session and each treatment session). Additionally, as the cross-sectional area may differ according to an individuals' overall size and age, Pearson correlation coefficients were calculated to assess the relationship between these factors at the time of screening.
The correlation between the response to angry ambiguous faces and the mean cross-sectional area of the nasal valve was determined after 8 IU-OT, 24 IU-OT, IV-OT and Placebo treatments. In this study, as administration was done to both the left and right nasal cavities, the mean cross-sectional areas were determined for each of the left and right nasal cavities, and a mean cross-sectional area was determined from the sum of these means for the left and right nasal cavities.
Bayes Factors using the Jeffreys-Zellner-Siow method60 were also calculated to assess the strength of evidence for the null and alternative hypotheses. This approach is especially useful in determining if the data supports the null hypotheses (i.e., no relationship between two variables) over the alternative hypothesis (i.e., there is a relationship between two variables), as a non-significant p-value is unable to provide evidence for the null-hypothesis85. A Bayes value less than ⅓ provides substantial evidence for the null hypothesis, over 3 provides strong evidence for the alternative hypothesis, and between ⅓ and 3 provides no strong support either way63.
Confidence intervals for the difference between correlations for each treatment condition were calculated to compare the strength of correlation to investigate whether the relationship between the mean cross-sectional area of the nasal valve and anger ratings of ambiguous faces is significantly greater than the relationships observed after the other treatments. As these variables are highly related due to measurements being taken from the same sample62, the CIs were adjusted to account for overlap58 using the Fisher Z transformation. Any CI interval that includes zero would indicate that the null hypothesis of no difference between the correlations could not be rejected.
The relationship between blood plasma and the mean cross-sectional area of the nasal valve was also calculated, as represented in Table 6. A change score between baseline OT and AVP and serum levels just before the social cognition assessment (˜40 minutes after treatment) was calculated to explore the effect of the cross-sectional area of the nasal valve on OT, AVP and cortisol on systemic availability.
A repeated-measures ANOVA revealed no main effect of time for the mean cross-sectional area of the nasal valve [F(1.99,29.86)=0.69, p=0.51; η2p=0.044]. There was also no relationship between age [r=0.56, 95% CI (−0.45, 0.54), n=16, p=0.84] and BMI [r=−0.68, 95% CI (−0.55, 0.44), n=15, p=0.015] with the mean cross-sectional area of the nasal valve at the time of screening.
The calculation of Pearson correlation coefficients revealed a significant relationship between the anger ratings of neutral faces and the mean cross-sectional area of the nasal valve after 8 IU-OT treatment [r=−0.61, 95% CI (−0.85, −0.14), n=15, p=0.015], with a corresponding Bayes factor (B) of 3.62, representing substantial evidence that these two variables are related. The relationship between angry ratings of ambiguous faces and the mean cross-sectional area of the nasal valve following the 8 IU-OT treatment is represented in
As represented in
A comparison of the correlation coefficients also revealed a significant difference between the correlations of the 8 IU-OT, and IV [r=−0.72 (−1.4, −0.2)] and Placebo [r=−0.65 (−1.1, −0.06)] treatments, but no significant difference in the correlation with 24 IU-OT treatment [r=−0.42 (−0.97, 0.06)].
In addition, there was no relationship between the cross-sectional area of the nasal valve and plasma concentration of OT, AVP, or cortisol after any of the treatment conditions.
The present study evidences that the efficacy of OT on social cognition can be influenced by control of the cross-sectional area of the nasal valve when intranasally administering a defined, lower-dosage of OT less than 24 IU. In one embodiment this control is obtained by the effective pressure of the exhaled air flow and the structural effect of the nosepiece in opening the nasal valve.
fMRI Analysis
Conventional fMRI pre-processing of the fMRI data was performed using independent component analysis (ICA) and auto-classification using the FMRIB's ICA-based X-noiseifier (FIX) method in order to de-noise the fMRI data.
The individual components were grouped using a temporal concatenation approach in MELODIC (Multivariate Exploratory Linear Optimised Decomposition into Independent Components), fixed model order at 40 components.
The component with strongest amygdala weighting (and also having strong medial temporal lobe (MTL) and brain stem weighting) was then determined, here Independent Component #37 (IC0037).
Dual regression was then performed to estimate the spatial maps of the individual components and the corresponding time courses, as represented in
Voxel-wise general linear model (GLM) testing was performed for evaluation of the main effect of the OT condition (F-test across the IU08-OT, IU24-OT, IV-OT and Placebo treatments) on the individual spatial maps within the canonical component (t>5) for IC0037. The largest clusters at voxel-wise p<0.01, uncorrected, were then identified. The two largest clusters showing the main effects of the OT condition are localized within the left and right amygdala, respectively, as represented in
Next, pairwise comparison between 8 IU-OT and Placebo treatments revealed two clusters showing significantly (p<0.05, cluster size corrected using permutation testing) increased connectivity in the 8 IU-OT treatment as compared to Placebo in the left and right amygdala, respectively, as represented in
A repeated-measures ANOVA was performed.
As expected, repeated-measures ANOVA revealed significant main effects of condition in both clusters (p=0.0032 and p=0.0039). Boxplots suggest that main effects of OT condition are driven by IU08-OT vs Placebo, indicating increased amygdala connectivity in the IU08-OT treatment, which is also supported by post-hoc pairwise comparisons (t=−2.54, p=0.016, and t=−2.24, p=0.033).
The amygdala is a key brain region for emotion regulation86, playing an important role in processing incoming social stimuli87. Indeed, converging neuroimaging evidence suggests the amygdala is an important target of OT administration. For instance, a single administration of intranasal OT has been reported to both decrease88-89 and increase90-91 amygdala activity when viewing a range of emotional stimuli. While these early studies measured neuronal recruitment during the presentation of stimuli, recent work has begun to explore brain activity at rest. It is reported that the amygdala is a key constituent of a larger “social brain network” that displays increased blood flow after OT administration92. Similarly, data indicates that OT administration increases connectivity between the amygdala and the rostral medial frontal cortex93.
The present study is the first to examine resting state connectivity after OT administration of different doses (8 IU and 24 IU) and treatment modalities (intranasal vs. intravenous). The data suggests that a low dose of OT delivered intranasally (but not intravenously) modulates amygdala connectivity, which is consistent with nose-to-brain delivery. Increased amygdala connectivity may facilitate the increased salience of social stimuli, which is suggested to underpin the observed effects of OT on social cognition and behavior10. These results may also have implications for the treatment of psychiatric disorders characterized by social impairment, which are also reported to have abnormal coupling between the amygdala and other brain regions (e.g., schizophrenia)94. Moreover, the data also adds to our understanding of how different OT doses and administration modalities influence neuronal recruitment at rest.
In summary, the present study presents new insights in relation to an improved method of deep intranasal OT delivery, and shows that greater pharmacodynamic activity can be shown specifically using the present delivery regime of OT as compared to IV delivery producing similar systemic exposure, suggesting that direct nose-to-brain activity is being achieved. This data also provides preliminary evidence that the selection of intranasal OT dose based on precedence, rather than experimental evidence, may be misguided; the current study indicating that a lower dose (8 IU) can offer greater efficacy than a higher dose (24 IU) when suitably administered.
MRI and Pupilometry Analysis
FreeSurfer (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu) was used for of the T1-weighted data, including surface reconstruction and full brain segmentation123 to obtain precise brain extracted volumes for co-registration of the fMRI data. FRRIB Software Library (FSL; http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/124) was used to process fMRI data. The first five volumes were discarded. Pre-processing of fMRI data was conducted using FMRIB's Expert Analysis Tool (FEAT) version 6.0128. This included motion correction using MCFLIRT124, spatial smoothing by means of SUSAN125 using a Gaussian kernel of FWHM of 7 mm, and a temporal high pass filter of 100 s. Single session independent component analysis (ICA) was performed using Multivariate Exploratory Linear Optimized Decomposition into Independent Components (MELODIC ICA126) in order to perform automated denoising (see below). FMRIB's Linear and non-linear Image Registration Tools (FLIRT124) optimized using Boundary Based Registration (BBR127) was used to align each participant's fMRI data to a standard space (MNI-152) with the T1-weighted volume as an intermediate.
Individual level general linear models (GLM) were fitted using FILM (FMRIB's Improved Linear Model)127-128 modeling the facial stimuli (happy/angry/ambiguous faces) and geometrical shape as events with the interspersed fixation trials as implicit baselines. Q1 and Q2 were modeled as one regressor across the different facial stimuli and shapes. Next, the average amygdala contrast-parameter estimates (COPE) were extracted from left and right amygdala masks based on the Harvard-Oxford anatomical atlas provided with FSL and submitted the values to higher-level linear mixed models in SPSS to test for main effects of condition and treatment (see below).
Pupilometry data was pre-processed using a custom made MATLAB-script. Raw data were converted into diameters, with physiologically unlikely pupil sizes (<2 mm or >9 mm) excluded from the data to remove noise (e.g., eye blinks). Each time series was split into trials with the average pupil diameter from each stimuli condition calculated. Finally, the first 8 seconds across all 20 trials for each condition were averaged to generate mean overall pupil diameters.
Statistical analysis was conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics version 22 (IBM, Armonk, N.Y.) to examine the impact of treatment on amygdala activity. As described above, a linear mixed-model (LMM) approach was adopted for the analysis of amygdala activity. All models were fitted using an unstructured matrix. Experimental treatment was both a fixed and repeated effect in the LMM testing the impact of treatment on amygdala activity. The same LMM approach was used to examine differences in mean pupil diameter, COPE values for contrasts of both left and right amygdala activity between angry faces and shapes, happy faces and shapes, and happy faces and angry faces. Standardized residuals after model fitting were examined for outliers. Z-scores above 2.58 or below −2.58 were removed from the analysis. Outliers beyond these thresholds were removed from the amygdala activation datasets (1 value from the right amygdala data during the presentation of angry, happy and, ambiguous, and shape stimuli, respectively; 1 value from left amygdala anger and happy data, respectively; and 2 values from the left amygdala ambiguous and shape data, respectively). For any significant main effects (p<0.05), post-hoc tests were performed to compare each treatment condition with the adjustment of critical p values to correct for multiple comparisons using a 5% false discovery rate (FDR)59. The relationships between amygdala activation and; mean pupil dilation, behavioral ratings, and nasal physiology were also assessed. Finally, Bayes Factors using the Jeffreys-Zellner-Siow prior60 were calculated to examine the strength of evidence for both the null and alternative hypotheses.
LMM revealed a significant main effect of treatment on right amygdala activity during the presentation of angry faces [F(3,15.1)=4.54, p=0.019;
There was no significant main effect of treatment on mean pupil diameter while processing angry [F(3,15)=0.57, p=0.64], happy [F(3,15)=0.62, p=0.62], or emotionally ambiguous faces [F(3,15)=1.33, p=0.3]. However, there was a significant relationship between right amygdala activation and mean pupil diameter during the presentation of, angry (p=0.02;
aN = 15,
bN = 16;
aN = 14,
bN = 15,
cN = 16;
aN = 15,
bN = 16;
In this study, 8 IU-OT treatment is shown to reduce amygdala activity in comparison to placebo. These findings are the first to report direct comparison of nose-to-brain and systemic delivery of OT, and indicate that OT delivery via nose-to-brain pathways—but not peripherally delivered OT producing similar blood levels—replicates a well-characterized finding of reduced right amygdala activation in response to emotional stimuli after OT treatment88,114-115.
Significantly, this data is consistent with the findings as discussed above that OT delivered by the inventive device modulates the perception of anger in facial stimuli and with animal models that associated a lower OT dose with stronger increases in social recognition69-70, which is pertinent given the important role of the amygdala in social cognition and behavior.
These effects may not be specific to negatively-valenced social stimuli as the main effects of treatment on right amygdala activity during the presentation of happy and ambiguous faces were significant and on the border of significance, respectively. Subsequent posthoc comparisons between the 8 IU-OT treatment and placebo were on the border of statistical significance. The observed reductions in right amygdala activity during the presentation of both positively and negatively valenced stimuli after OT treatment are consistent with the hypothesis that OT increases approach-related behaviours114,118.
Secondary analysis revealed a significant association between right amygdala activity and mean pupil diameter during the processing of angry, ambiguous, and happy facial stimuli after 8 IU-OT administration. While a main effect of treatment on pupil diameter not was found, the data is indicative of the amygdala modulating cognitive resources to facial stimuli, regardless of valence, after 8 IU-OT treatment.
The amygdala is a site of large number of oxytocin receptorst131-132. These receptors have been shown to operate by inhibiting amygdala activity via the increase of GABAergic interneuron activity133-134. The observed decrease in amygdala activity after OT administration using the inventive device is consistent with nose-to-brain molecule transport via olfactory and trigeminal nerve fiber pathways135. Outputs to the amygdala via the olfactory bulbs136-138 or transport through brain extracellular fluid139 from olfactory bulb and brainstem delivery sites may facilitate these reductions in amygdala activity via a local GABAergic circuit after intranasal delivery. Irrespective of how endogenous OT precisely affects amygdala activity, by having a peripheral comparator this study demonstrates that nose-to-brain pathways produce effects not observed with comparable levels of purely systemic exposure, suggesting facilitated entry to the brain.
The dose-response data reported here suggest that a low dose of OT delivered using the inventive device is sufficient to modulate amygdala activity. Patients with underlying deficits responsive to OT may respond more robustly than healthy volunteers.
There are a number of reasons that may explain why an effect was found with the 8 IU-OT dose but not the 24 IU-OT. These include cross reactivity with vasopressin receptors49 and the possibility that an 8 IU-OT dose delivered with the inventive device is better able to reach the regions in the nose where direct nose-to-brain transport can occur.
Significantly, no evidence was found that 1 IU-OT of peripherally administered OT influences amygdala activity. Although there is conflicting evidence on whether peripheral OT can cross the BBB140-141, our study suggests that even if OT does travel across this barrier in small amounts, this quantity is not large enough to modulate amygdala activity compared to placebo. Individual differences and context can influence the response to OT administration16, thus a strength of this study was the use of a within-subjects design to examine amygdala activity. By adopting this experimental design, any individual differences due to variation in the endogenous oxytocin system142-143 are minimized.
In summary, the present study shows surprisingly that a low dose of OT intranasally delivered with the described delivery method modulates amygdala activity, and this result provides additional evidence to suggest a lower intranasal OT dose may better facilitate the modulation of social cognition and behavior and that peripheral actions of OT do not appear to have any significant neural corollaries.
This application claims the benefit of and priority to U.S. Provisional Application No. 62/081,742, filed Nov. 19, 2014. The entire disclosure of which is incorporated herein by reference in its entirety.
Number | Name | Date | Kind |
---|---|---|---|
605436 | Kellogg | Jun 1898 | A |
642748 | Manners | Feb 1900 | A |
746749 | Seidel | Dec 1903 | A |
5797392 | Keldmann et al. | Aug 1998 | A |
5901703 | Ohki et al. | May 1999 | A |
6648848 | Keldmann et al. | Nov 2003 | B1 |
6715485 | Djupesland | Apr 2004 | B1 |
D530815 | Murphy et al. | Oct 2006 | S |
7347201 | Djupesland | Mar 2008 | B2 |
7377901 | Djupesland et al. | May 2008 | B2 |
7481218 | Djupesland | Jan 2009 | B2 |
7543581 | Djupesland | Jun 2009 | B2 |
7740014 | Djupesland | Jun 2010 | B2 |
7784460 | Djupesland et al. | Aug 2010 | B2 |
7841337 | Djupesland | Nov 2010 | B2 |
7854227 | Djupesland | Dec 2010 | B2 |
7934503 | Djupesland et al. | May 2011 | B2 |
7975690 | Djupesland | Jun 2011 | B2 |
8047202 | Djupesland | Nov 2011 | B2 |
8146589 | Djupesland | Apr 2012 | B2 |
8171929 | Djupesland et al. | May 2012 | B2 |
8198240 | Yeomans et al. | Jun 2012 | B2 |
8327844 | Djupesland | Dec 2012 | B2 |
8511303 | Djupesland | Aug 2013 | B2 |
8522778 | Djupesland | Sep 2013 | B2 |
8550073 | Djupesland | Oct 2013 | B2 |
8555877 | Djupesland | Oct 2013 | B2 |
8555878 | Djupesland | Oct 2013 | B2 |
8590530 | Djupesland et al. | Nov 2013 | B2 |
8596278 | Djupesland | Dec 2013 | B2 |
8800555 | Djupesland | Aug 2014 | B2 |
8875704 | Djupesland et al. | Nov 2014 | B2 |
8899229 | Djupesland et al. | Dec 2014 | B2 |
8910629 | Djupesland et al. | Dec 2014 | B2 |
D723156 | Djupesland et al. | Feb 2015 | S |
D725769 | Djupesland et al. | Mar 2015 | S |
8978647 | Djupesland et al. | Mar 2015 | B2 |
9010325 | Djupesland et al. | Apr 2015 | B2 |
9038630 | Djupesland et al. | May 2015 | B2 |
9067034 | Djupesland et al. | Jun 2015 | B2 |
9072857 | Djupesland | Jul 2015 | B2 |
9108015 | Djupesland | Aug 2015 | B2 |
9119932 | Djupesland | Sep 2015 | B2 |
9132249 | Djupesland | Sep 2015 | B2 |
9144652 | Djupesland et al. | Sep 2015 | B2 |
9168341 | Djupesland | Oct 2015 | B2 |
9205208 | Djupesland | Dec 2015 | B2 |
9205209 | Djupesland | Dec 2015 | B2 |
20040024330 | Djupesland et al. | Feb 2004 | A1 |
20040112378 | Djupesland | Jun 2004 | A1 |
20040112379 | Djupesland | Jun 2004 | A1 |
20040112380 | Djupesland | Jun 2004 | A1 |
20040149289 | Djupesland | Aug 2004 | A1 |
20040182388 | Djupesland | Sep 2004 | A1 |
20040235956 | Quay | Nov 2004 | A1 |
20050028812 | Djupesland | Feb 2005 | A1 |
20050072430 | Djupesland | Apr 2005 | A1 |
20050235992 | Djupesland | Oct 2005 | A1 |
20060096589 | Djupesland | May 2006 | A1 |
20060107957 | Djupesland | May 2006 | A1 |
20060169278 | Djupesland et al. | Aug 2006 | A1 |
20060219240 | Djupesland | Oct 2006 | A1 |
20060219241 | Djupesland | Oct 2006 | A1 |
20060225732 | Djupesland | Oct 2006 | A1 |
20060231094 | Djupesland | Oct 2006 | A1 |
20070039614 | Djupesland | Feb 2007 | A1 |
20070054843 | Yeomans et al. | Mar 2007 | A1 |
20070125371 | Djupesland | Jun 2007 | A1 |
20070186927 | Djupesland et al. | Aug 2007 | A1 |
20080161771 | Djupesland | Jul 2008 | A1 |
20080163874 | Djupesland | Jul 2008 | A1 |
20080221471 | Djupesland et al. | Sep 2008 | A1 |
20080223363 | Djupesland | Sep 2008 | A1 |
20080289629 | Djupesland et al. | Nov 2008 | A1 |
20090101146 | Djupesland | Apr 2009 | A1 |
20090181880 | Yeomans et al. | Jul 2009 | A1 |
20090293873 | Djupesland et al. | Dec 2009 | A1 |
20090304802 | Djupesland et al. | Dec 2009 | A1 |
20090314293 | Djupesland | Dec 2009 | A1 |
20090320832 | Djupesland | Dec 2009 | A1 |
20100035805 | Hafner | Feb 2010 | A1 |
20100051022 | Djupesland et al. | Mar 2010 | A1 |
20100057047 | Djupesland et al. | Mar 2010 | A1 |
20100147292 | Hamaguchi et al. | Jun 2010 | A1 |
20100242959 | Djupesland et al. | Sep 2010 | A1 |
20100282246 | Djupesland | Nov 2010 | A1 |
20100288275 | Djupesland et al. | Nov 2010 | A1 |
20100300439 | Djupesland et al. | Dec 2010 | A1 |
20110023869 | Djupesland | Feb 2011 | A1 |
20110053827 | Hafner | Mar 2011 | A1 |
20110088690 | Djupesland et al. | Apr 2011 | A1 |
20110088691 | Djupesland | Apr 2011 | A1 |
20110114087 | Djupesland et al. | May 2011 | A1 |
20110126830 | Djupesland et al. | Jun 2011 | A1 |
20110259329 | Djupesland et al. | Oct 2011 | A1 |
20110318345 | Djupesland | Dec 2011 | A1 |
20120000459 | Djupesland | Jan 2012 | A1 |
20120006323 | Djupesland | Jan 2012 | A1 |
20120073571 | Djupesland | Mar 2012 | A1 |
20120090608 | Djupesland et al. | Apr 2012 | A1 |
20120260915 | Djupesland | Oct 2012 | A1 |
20130098362 | Djupesland et al. | Apr 2013 | A1 |
20130125889 | Djupesland et al. | May 2013 | A1 |
20130327320 | Djupesland | Dec 2013 | A1 |
20140018295 | Djupesland | Jan 2014 | A1 |
20140041660 | Djupesland et al. | Feb 2014 | A1 |
20140060536 | Djupesland | Mar 2014 | A1 |
20140073562 | Djupesland | Mar 2014 | A1 |
20140144442 | Djupesland et al. | May 2014 | A1 |
20140144443 | Djupesland et al. | May 2014 | A1 |
20140166008 | Djupesland | Jun 2014 | A1 |
20140202456 | Djupesland | Jul 2014 | A1 |
20140246022 | Djupesland et al. | Sep 2014 | A1 |
20150007811 | Djupesland et al. | Jan 2015 | A1 |
20150013670 | Djupesland et al. | Jan 2015 | A1 |
20150013677 | Djupesland et al. | Jan 2015 | A1 |
20150053201 | Djupesland et al. | Feb 2015 | A1 |
20150090259 | Djupesland et al. | Apr 2015 | A1 |
20150101605 | Djupesland et al. | Apr 2015 | A1 |
20150144129 | Djupesland et al. | May 2015 | A1 |
20150182709 | Djupesland | Jul 2015 | A1 |
20150246194 | Djupesland et al. | Sep 2015 | A1 |
20150367090 | Djupesland et al. | Dec 2015 | A1 |
20150367091 | Djupesland et al. | Dec 2015 | A1 |
20160001022 | Djupesland et al. | Jan 2016 | A1 |
Number | Date | Country |
---|---|---|
1146729 | Apr 1997 | CN |
101056666 | Oct 2007 | CN |
101801446 | Aug 2010 | CN |
101918061 | Dec 2010 | CN |
2 400 565 | Oct 2004 | GB |
2 437 488 | Oct 2007 | GB |
2 438 834 | Dec 2007 | GB |
09-135901 | May 1997 | JP |
2007-528248 | Oct 2007 | JP |
2008-62974 | Mar 2008 | JP |
2011-511674 | Apr 2011 | JP |
2 383 358 | Nov 2009 | RU |
112 044 | Jan 2012 | RU |
WO 9622802 | Aug 1996 | WO |
WO 9853869 | Dec 1998 | WO |
WO 9913930 | Mar 1999 | WO |
WO 0051672 | Sep 2000 | WO |
WO 0197689 | Dec 2001 | WO |
WO 02068029 | Sep 2002 | WO |
WO 02068030 | Sep 2002 | WO |
WO 02068031 | Sep 2002 | WO |
WO 02068032 | Sep 2002 | WO |
WO 03000310 | Jan 2003 | WO |
WO 03020350 | Mar 2003 | WO |
WO 03082393 | Oct 2003 | WO |
WO 03084591 | Oct 2003 | WO |
WO 03090812 | Nov 2003 | WO |
WO 2004004814 | Jan 2004 | WO |
WO 2004004922 | Jan 2004 | WO |
WO 2004060433 | Jul 2004 | WO |
WO 2004103447 | Dec 2004 | WO |
WO 2005016423 | Feb 2005 | WO |
WO 2005021059 | Mar 2005 | WO |
WO 2005087615 | Sep 2005 | WO |
WO 2006030210 | Mar 2006 | WO |
WO 2006090149 | Aug 2006 | WO |
WO 2007083073 | Jul 2007 | WO |
WO 2007093784 | Aug 2007 | WO |
WO 2007093791 | Aug 2007 | WO |
WO 2007099361 | Sep 2007 | WO |
WO 2007102089 | Sep 2007 | WO |
WO 2007107887 | Sep 2007 | WO |
WO 2007125318 | Nov 2007 | WO |
WO 2007141541 | Dec 2007 | WO |
WO 2008012531 | Jan 2008 | WO |
WO 2008042452 | Apr 2008 | WO |
WO 2008065403 | Jun 2008 | WO |
WO 2008081326 | Jul 2008 | WO |
WO 2008081327 | Jul 2008 | WO |
WO 2008122791 | Oct 2008 | WO |
WO 2008122795 | Oct 2008 | WO |
WO 2009012137 | Jan 2009 | WO |
WO 2009044172 | Apr 2009 | WO |
WO 2009100383 | Aug 2009 | WO |
WO 2010029441 | Mar 2010 | WO |
WO 2012035427 | Mar 2012 | WO |
WO 2012123819 | Sep 2012 | WO |
WO 2013124491 | Aug 2013 | WO |
WO 2013124492 | Aug 2013 | WO |
WO 2013124493 | Aug 2013 | WO |
WO 2014155192 | Oct 2014 | WO |
Entry |
---|
Cindy H. Dubin, Nothing to Sneeze At, Pharmaceutical Formulation & Quality Magazine (Jan. 29, 2003). |
Per Gisle Djupesland, Nasal Delivery of Vaccines, EPC (Jan. 29, 2003). |
Per Gisle Djupesland, Who Nose How Far Nasal Delivery Can Go?, EPC (Oct. 7, 2003). |
Per Gisle Djupesland, Bi-directional Nasal Drug Delivery, Innovations in Pharmaceutical Technology (Jul. 10, 2004). |
P.G. Djupesland, Bi-Directional Nasal Delivery of Aerosols Can Prevent Lung Deposition, Journal of Aerosol Medicine (Sep. 2004). |
Bi-Directional Nasal Device Delivers Drug on Exhalation, Pharmaceutical Technology (Sep. 10, 2004). |
Ola Dale et al., Intranasal Midazolam: A Comparison of Two Delivery Devices in Human Volunteers, Journal of Pharmacy and Pharmacology (Oct. 2004). |
M. Kleven, Using Computational Fluid Dynamics (CFD) to Improve the Bi-Directional Nasal Drug Delivery Concept, Trans IChemE Part C. (Jun. 2005). |
Per Gisle Djupesland, Breath-Actuated Bi-Directional Delivery Sets the Nasal Market on a New Course, ONdrugDelivery (Oct. 10, 2005). |
Hilde Bakke et al., Oral Spray Immunization May be an Alternative to Intranasal Vaccine Delivery to Induce Systemic Antibodies But Not Nasal Mucosal or Cellular Immunity, Scan J. of Immunol. (Mar. 2006). |
P.G. Djupesland et al., Breath Actuated Nasal Device Improves Delivery to Target Sites Beyond the Nasal Valve, The Laryngoscope (Mar. 2006). |
R. Luthringer et al., Rapid Absorption of Sumatriptan Powder and Effects on Glyceryl tinitrate Model of Headache Following Intranasal Delivery Using a Novel Bi-Directional Device, Journal of Pharmacy and Pharmacology (Jan. 2009). |
A. Skretting et al., A New Method for Scintigraphic Quantification of Deposition and Clearance in Anatomical Regions of the Human Nose, Nuclear Medicine Communications (Aug. 2009). |
Vlckovia et al., Effective Treatment of Mild-to-Moderate Nasal Polyposis with Fluticasone Delivered by a Novel Device, Rhinology (Oct. 22, 2009). |
Per Gisle Djupesland et al., Impact of Baseline Nasal Polyp Size and Previous Surgery on Efficacy of Fluticasone Delivered With a Novel Device: A Subgroup Analysis, Am. J. Rhinology Allergy (2010). |
P.G. Djupesland et al., Intranasal Sumatriptan Powder Delivered by a Novel Breath Actuated Bi-Directional Device for the Acute Treatment of Migraine: A Randomised Placebo-Controlled Study, Cephalalgia (Mar. 17, 2010). |
F.S. Hansen et al., Preliminary Efficacy of Fluticasone Delivered by a Novel Device in Recalcitrant Chronic Rhinosinusitis, Rhinology (Jun. 26, 2010). |
Per Gisle Djupesland, Nasal Drug Delivery Devices: Characteristics and Performance in Clinical Perspective—A Review, Drug. Deliv. and Transl. Res. (Oct. 18, 2012). |
Per Gisle Djupesland, Nasal Deposition and Clearance in Man: Comparison of a Bidirectional Powder Device and a Traditional Liquid Spray Pump, Journal of Aerosol Medicine and Pulmonary Drug Delivery (Nov. 2012). |
Stewart J. Tepper, Clinical Implications for Breath-Powered Powder Sumatriptan Intranasal Treatment, Headache, The American Headache Society (Apr. 29, 2013). |
Mohammad Obaidi et al., Improved Pharmacokinetics of Sumatriptan With Breath Powered Nasal Delivery of Sumatriptan Powder, Headache, The American Headache Society (May 24, 2013). |
Per Gisle Djupesland, Breath Powdered Nasal Delivery: A New Route to Rapid Headache Relief, Headache, The American Headache Society (Jun. 4, 2013). |
Per Gisle Djupesland et al., The Nasal Approach to Delivering Treatment for Brain Diseases: An Anatomic, Physiologic, and Delivery Technology Overview, Therapeutic Delivery (2014). |
R.K. Cady et al., A Randomized Double-Blind, Placebo Controlled Study of Breath Powered Nasal Delivery of Sumatriptan Powder (AVP-825) in the Treatment of Acute Migraine (The TARGET Study), Headache (Sep. 8, 2014). |
S.J. Tepper et al., AVP-825 Breath-Powdered Intranasal Delivery System Containing 22 mg Sumatriptan Powder vs. 100 mg Oral Sumatripta in the Acute Treatment of Migraines (The COMPASS Study): A Comparative Randomized Clinical Trial Across Multiple Attacks, Headache: The Journal of Head and Face Pain (Mar. 29, 2015). |
D. S. Quintana et al., Low-dose Oxytocin Delivered Intranasally with Breath Powdered Device Affects Social-Cognitive Behavior: A Randomized Four-Way Crossover Trial with Nasal Cavity Dimension Assessment, Transl Psychiatry (Jul. 14, 2015). |
R. Mahmoud, Breathe Out, Innovations in Phar, Tech. (Dec. 10, 2015). |
Epperson et al., Intranasal Oxytocin in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder, Biol Psychiatry, 40:547-49 (1996). |
Kovács et al., Oxytocin and Addiction: A Review, Psychoneuroendocrinology, 23(8):945-62 (1998). |
Ross et al., Oxytocin and the neural mechanisms regulating social cognition and affiliative behavior, Front Neuroendocrinol, 30(4):534-547 (Oct. 2009). |
ClinicalTrials.gov archive, NCT01028677, Oxytocin Treatment of Schizophrenia (Dec. 8, 2009). |
ClinicalTrials.gov archive, NCT01983514, Effects of Intranasal Administration of a Single Dose of Oxytocin Using a Novel Device in Healthy Adults (Apr. 23, 2014). |
Guastella et al., Intranasal oxytocin improves emotion recognition for youth with autism spectrum disorders, 67(7):692-694 (2010). |
Oxytocin IU, Oxytocin Leaflet (Sep. 2012). |
Number | Date | Country | |
---|---|---|---|
20160331916 A1 | Nov 2016 | US |
Number | Date | Country | |
---|---|---|---|
62081742 | Nov 2014 | US |